Wk8 Discussion SOCW 6443: The Use of Stimulants in the Treatment ADHD

  

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

Wk8 Discussion SOCW 6443: The Use of Stimulants in the Treatment ADHD

Stimulant medications have been used since the mid-1900s to treat ADHD. More recently, medical professionals recognize a degree of complicity in the development of addiction in many of their clients with ADHD in connection with the use of stimulant treatment medications such as dextroamphetamine, levoamphetamine, and methylphenidate. As members of medical treatment teams, mental health professionals recognize their responsibility to treat current clients who have developed this comorbid condition as well as to work to prevent future problems of addiction that might arise from use of stimulant treatment. Clients deserve encompassing, accurate information in order to sort through potentially mixed messages from medical professionals, school personnel, family members, and others. Mental health professionals can provide or point to educational materials to help clients make informed choices, provide information about alternatives based on sound research, and help monitor for misuse of potentially addictive medications (Preston, O’Neal, & Talaga, 2013).

For this Discussion, review the media titled “Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Counseling Session” and consider the medications a psychiatrist might prescribe to treat ADHD. Conduct an Internet search or a Walden Library search for at least one peer-reviewed journal article that addresses issues related to the use of stimulants for the treatment of ADHD as it relates to the client in the media program.

All questions in bold then the answers 300 to 500 words not including the questions

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

The “Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Counseling Session” transcript has been uploaded 

Must use and reference the DSM-5

Post a brief description of the client’s current presentation in the media program. 

Choose a medication that a psychiatrist might prescribe to treat ADHD and explain in detail the major action, intended effects, neurotransmitters implicated in its use, and side effects. 

Explain why you think this medication might be the most effective for treatment. 

Explain how you might address any issues related to the use of stimulants for the treatment of ADHD. Justify your choice based on the client’s presentation and support your position with the Learning Resources and your journal article. 

Explain an alternative approach to treating ADHD.

Be sure to support your postings and responses with specific references to the Learning Resources in text citations and full references APA 7th addition format

Recourses

Lichtblau, L. (2011). Psychopharmacology demystified. Clifton Park, NY: Delmar, Cengage Learning.

Chapter 5, “Cognitive Enhancers” (pp. 65–74)

Preston, J. D., O’Neal, J. H., & Talaga, M. C. (2017). Handbook of clinical psychopharmacology for therapists (8th ed.). Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.

Chapter 23, “Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology” (pp. 255-276)

Attention Deficit: Hyperactivity Disorder Counseling Session

Attention Deficit: Hyperactivity Disorder Counseling Session
Program Transcript

[MUSIC PLAYING]

TANYA: Oh! I used to have one of these action figures at home. That’s cool.
Have you ever played Halo? How about Grand Theft Auto? That’s another one of
my favorite games. Do you have any good games we can play here?

COUNSELOR: Well, Tanya, I love your energy. And I have this pink Play-Doh
that has never been touched. And so I’m wondering if we could talk for a couple
of minutes, and then maybe we could get to some games later. Would you like to
give that a try?

TANYA: OK. Cool.

COUNSELOR: I’m going to get some, too. I think I’m going to get the other pink.
And so you and your mom and dad and I met for a few minutes before. And we
decided that it would be good for you and I to have some time together, just to
spend time.

We can do some playing. We can hang out with each other and talk. And we can
do Play-Doh. And so we can do all sorts of things.

But the thing I want you to know first, before we do anything else, is that what
you say in here stays in here. It’s private. It’s our stuff we’re talking about.

Now, your mom and dad and I will have some conversations. But I won’t say
anything about you behind your back to them. And I do have to let them know if
you were going to do something dangerous. We would talk about that, obviously,
but not that I think that’s the case. Does that make sense? So now, let’s talk
about what kinds of things you would like to have better in your life.

TANYA: What do you mean?

COUNSELOR: Well, let’s say you had three wishes. And you can make three
wishes, one about yourself, one about school, one about home. What would you
like to have different?

TANYA: Can I wish for more wishes?

COUNSELOR: No, but that’s a very smart question to ask, because if you could,
you would have wishes forever. So we have a limit on that. So it’s just really only
three, one about you, one about school, and one about home.

© 2014 Laureate Education, Inc. 1

Attention Deficit: Hyperactivity Disorder Counseling Session

TANYA: Well, I wish I would never have any more homework. And I wish my
parents would get off my case about playing too many computer games. And I
wish I could be invisible and get away with whatever I want without annoying
anybody.

COUNSELOR: Wow, that is a perfect response. So you’d get rid of homework.
You’d make it so your parents wouldn’t be on your case about video or computer
games. And you would be invisible. You could get away with anything.

So Tanya, I’m just interested. It sounds like you would use a whole wish—and
you’ve only got three—on homework.

TANYA: Yep, no homework the rest of my life and my life would be way better.

COUNSELOR: Getting rid of homework would make your life better. And that
makes me think maybe homework is pretty miserable and feels awful right now.

FEMALE SPEAKER: Yep. So poof. I make it disappear.

Attention Deficit: Hyperactivity Disorder Counseling Session
Additional Content Attribution

Creative Support Services
Los Angeles, CA

Dimension Sound Effects Library
Newnan, GA

Narrator Tracks Music Library
Stevens Point, WI

Signature Music, Inc.
Chesterton, IN

Studio Cutz Music Library
Carrollton, TX

© 2014 Laureate Education, Inc. 2

ARTICLEPEDIATRICS Volume 138 , number 3 , September 2016 :e 20160407

Racial and Ethnic Disparities in
ADHD Diagnosis and Treatment
Tumaini R. Coker, MD, MBA, a, b Marc N. Elliott, PhD, b Sara L. Toomey, MD, MPhil, MPH, MSc, c
David C. Schwebel, PhD, d Paula Cuccaro, PhD, e Susan Tortolero Emery, PhD, e Susan L.
Davies, PhD, f Susanna N. Visser, DrPH, MS, g Mark A. Schuster, MD, PhDb, c

abstractOBJECTIVES: We examined racial/ethnic disparities in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD) diagnosis and medication use and determined whether medication disparities

were more likely due to underdiagnosis or undertreatment of

African-American and Latino

children, or overdiagnosis

or overtreatment of white children.

METHODS: We used a population-based, multisite sample of 4297 children and parents

surveyed over 3 waves (fifth, seventh, and 10th grades). Multivariate logistic regression

examined disparities in parent-reported ADHD

diagnosis and medication use in the

following analyses: (1) using the total sample; (2) limited to children with an ADHD

diagnosis or symptoms; and (3) limited to children without a diagnosis or symptoms.

RESULTS: Across all waves, African-American and Latino children, compared with white

children, had lower odds of having an ADHD diagnosis and of taking ADHD medication,

controlling for sociodemographics, ADHD symptoms, and other potential comorbid mental

health symptoms. Among children with an ADHD diagnosis or symptoms, African-American

children had lower odds of medication use at fifth, seventh, and 10th grades, and Latino

children had lower odds at fifth and 10th grades. Among children who had neither ADHD

symptoms nor ADHD diagnosis by fifth grade (and thus would not likely meet ADHD

diagnostic criteria at any age), medication use did not vary by race/ethnicity in adjusted

analysis.

CONCLUSIONS: Racial/ethnic disparities in parent-reported medication use for ADHD are

robust, persisting from fifth grade to 10th grade. These findings suggest that disparities

may be more likely related to underdiagnosis and undertreatment of African-American and

Latino children as opposed to

overdiagnosis or overtreatment of white children.

aDepartment of Pediatrics, Mattel Children’s Hospital, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles,

California; bRAND, Santa Monica, California; cDivision of General Pediatrics, Boston Children’s Hospital and

Department of Pediatrics, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts; Departments of dPsychology and
fHealth Behavior, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, Alabama; eCenter for Health Promotion

and Prevention Research, University of Texas–Houston, School of Public Health, Houston, Texas; and gCenters

for Disease Control and Prevention, Atlanta, Georgia

Dr Coker was responsible for study conception and design, data analysis, interpretation of

fi ndings, and writing of the manuscript; Dr Elliott contributed to study conception and survey

development, obtained funding, and participated in study design, analysis, and interpretation;

he also revised manuscript drafts; Dr Schwebel contributed to study conception and survey

development, obtained funding, participated in study design and interpretation, and revised

manuscript drafts; Drs Toomey, Tortolero Emery, Cuccaro, and Davies contributed to study

conception and survey development, obtained funding, participated in study design, and revised

manuscript drafts; Dr Visser contributed to study conception, study design, and revision of

manuscript drafts; and Dr Schuster contributed to study conception and survey development,

To cite: Coker TR, Elliott MN, Toomey SL, et al. Racial
and Ethnic Disparities in ADHD Diagnosis and Treatment.

Pediatrics. 2016;138(3):e2016040

7

WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: There are
racial/ethnic disparities in medication use for

attention-defi cit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), but

it is unknown if the disparity is more likely due to

an underdiagnosis

or undertreatment of African-

American and Latino children, or an overdiagnosis

or overtreatment of white children.

WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: Racial/ethnic disparities
in medication use for ADHD are robust, persist

from fi fth to 10th grade, and seem to be more

related to underdiagnosis and undertreatment of

African-American and Latino children as opposed to

overdiagnosis or overtreatment of white children.

by guest on October 15, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

COKER et al

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder (ADHD) diagnoses have

been increasing in the United States.

Parent-reported rates of ever

receiving a diagnosis for children

aged 4 to 17 years increased from

7.8% in 2003 to 11.0% in 2011,

and rates of ADHD medication use

increased from 4.8% in 2007 to

6.1% in 2011. 1 Studies also describe

racial/ethnic disparities in diagnosis

and medical treatment of ADHD,

indicating that African-American

and Latino children may have lower

rates of receiving a diagnosis and

medication compared with white

children. 2 – 7

These differences in diagnosis and

treatment are generally interpreted

as reflecting underdiagnosis and

undertreatment of African-American

and Latino children. 5, 6 In light of

the increasing prevalence, however,

researchers have recognized that

overdiagnosis or overtreatment

of white children is a possible

alternative explanation for the

disparity, 3, 8 although previous

studies have not examined which

explanation is most likely.

The current study was conducted

to help address this question: Is

the disparity in ADHD diagnosis

and medication treatment more

likely due to an underdiagnosis

or undertreatment of African-

American and Latino children or

an overdiagnosis or overtreatment

of white children? A population-

based, multisite longitudinal survey

was used to examine racial/ethnic

disparities in the diagnosis of ADHD

and in ADHD medication treatment

among children. We also examined

whether the disparity and the likely

main drivers of the disparity changed

from fifth grade to 10th grade.

METHODS

Healthy Passages is a longitudinal

study of a cohort of 5147 fifth-

graders and their parents (2004–

2006), with follow-up in seventh

grade (2006–2008) and 10th grade

(2009–2011). 9, 10 Institutional review

board approval was obtained at each

study site and the Centers for Disease

Prevention and Control.

Study Population and Sampling
Procedure

Participants were recruited from

public schools in the following

districts: 10 contiguous public school

districts in and around Birmingham,

Alabama; 25 contiguous public school

districts in Los Angeles County,

California; and the largest public

school district in Houston, Texas.

Eligible schools had an enrollment

of ≥25 fifth-graders, representing
>99% of students enrolled in regular

classrooms. To ensure adequate

sample sizes of African-American,

Latino, and white students, a 2-stage

probability sampling procedure,

detailed elsewhere, 9 was used. The

sampling procedure included the

following: (1) random sampling of

schools using probabilities that were

a function of how closely a school’s

racial/ethnic mix corresponded to

the site’s racial/ethnic target; and (2)

invitation to participate to all fifth-

grade students in regular classrooms

of sampled schools.

The 118 sampled schools had 11 532

enrolled fifth-graders. A primary

caregiver (henceforth referred to as

“parent”) for each student received

a letter requesting permission for

contact by study personnel. Of the

11 532 parents, 6663 who either

agreed to be contacted or who were

unsure were invited to participate;

5147 completed an interview at

baseline (fifth grade), and 4297

parent–child dyads participated in

all 3 waves (at fifth grade and ∼2 and
5 years later, when most children

were in seventh and 10th grades,

respectively).

Our sample size reached the

predetermined sample size targets;

details of statistical power are

described elsewhere. 9 Interviews

were conducted at the home, a study

center, or another preferred location.

Parents provided informed consent

for participation, and children gave

assent.

Measures

ADHD Symptoms

Questions from the Diagnostic

Interview

Schedule for Children

Predictive Scales (DPS) were used

to assess the presence of parent-

reported symptoms of ADHD and

other mental health conditions that

may be comorbidities which could

affect whether a child receives a

diagnosis or medication for ADHD.

These comorbidities included

oppositional defiant disorder,

conduct disorder, and depression.

The DPS is a screening tool based on

the Diagnostic Interview Schedule

for Children; it relies on parent-

reported symptoms (reported as

present or not) of ADHD (7 yes/

no items), oppositional defiant

disorder (12 yes/no items), and

conduct disorder (8 yes/no items),

as well as child-reported symptoms

of depression (6 yes/no items)

during the previous 12 months

(sensitivities and specificities for

ADHD, oppositional defiant disorder,

conduct disorder, and depression,

≥0.89). 11 The 7 ADHD symptoms
in the DPS align with 5 inattentive

symptoms (eg, Has your child often

had trouble finishing his or her

homework or other things he or she

is supposed to?) and 2 hyperactivity/

impulsivity symptoms (eg, Has your

child often left his or her seat when

he or she was not supposed to?) on

the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition. 12
A dichotomous variable was created

for symptoms consistent with

ADHD, defined by a score (sum of

symptoms) above the sample 90th

percentile. We used this cutoff value,

which was more stringent than cutoff

values used in a previously studied

community sample, 11 because data

on level of impairment or symptom

severity were not collected. For

2
by guest on October 15, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

PEDIATRICS Volume 138 , number 3 , September 2016

fifth- and seventh-grade surveys,

the 90th percentile corresponds

to positive responses on ≥6 of 7
possible ADHD symptoms. For

the 10th-grade surveys, the 90th

percentile corresponds to ≥5 of 7
possible symptoms. Of note, the

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition,
requires ≥6 symptoms of inattention
and/or hyperactivity-impulsivity for

youth aged ≤16 years or ≥5 for youth
aged ≥17 years. 12 We also created a
continuous variable for symptoms

consistent with ADHD and each of

the other mental health disorders,

defined by the total symptom score

for each scale. Higher symptom

scores reflect more symptoms.

ADHD Diagnosis

In survey waves 1 and 3, parents

were asked if a physician or health

professional had ever told them

that their child had hyperactivity or

attention-deficit disorder (wave 1

question) or hyperactivity, attention-

deficit disorder, or ADHD (wave 3

question) (response options, yes or

no). This question was not asked in

wave 2.

ADHD Medication Use

In waves 1 through 3, parents were

asked if during the last year (wave 1)

or past 12 months (waves 2 and 3)

their child had taken medication for

being overactive, being hyperactive,

or having trouble paying attention

(yes or no).

Other Variables

Data were collected on several child

and parent wave 1 characteristics

previously hypothesized to influence

mental health care use. 13 – 15 Child

sociodemographic covariates

included study city (Birmingham,

Houston, and Los Angeles), child

race/ethnicity (non-Latino black

[henceforth, African-American],

Latino, non-Latino white, and other

race/ethnicity), age at fifth grade

survey (<11, 11, and ≥12 years), sex (male or female), insurance status

(uninsured or insured), annual

household income (less than $20 000,

$20 000–$34 000, $35 000–$69 999,

$70 000 or higher), and household

composition (2-parent, 1-parent, or

other). Parent sociodemographic

covariates included highest

household education level (no high

school diploma, high school diploma,

some college, and college degree

or greater) and English language

proficiency (speaks English very well

versus less than very well). Because

no significant differences were

found in results when accounting for

household size in the income variable

(by using the federal poverty level),

we used annual household income.

We also included child symptoms

of oppositional defiant disorder,

conduct disorder, and depression

(each as continuous variables),

and the child’s school functioning,

using the Pediatric Quality of Life

Inventory version 4.0 at each wave.

This inventory tool is a well-validated

instrument designed to measure

health-related quality of life in 2- to

18-year-olds. 16 It measures school

functioning by using 5 child-reported

items (hard to pay attention in class,

forgets things, trouble keeping up

with school work, missed school

because not feeling well, and missed

school to go to physician/hospital);

respondents report how much of a

problem each item has been during

the past month, with 5 response

options (never, almost never,

sometimes, often, and almost always

a problem). Items are reverse scored

(ie, higher scores represent better

school functioning) and linearly

transformed to a 0 to 100 range.

We included a dichotomous measure

of receipt of family-centered care

(FCC) collected by wave 3 parental

report. FCC is a key element of

the medical home, is less likely to

be reported by African-American

and Latino parents, and may be

associated with having fewer unmet

medical needs. 17 – 19 Although FCC

was only measured in wave 3, it was

used as a covariate in analyses of

all waves, as a general indicator of

access to FCC. FCC was indicated as

received if the parent reported that

their child’s physicians “always”

or “usually” spent enough time,

listened carefully, were sensitive

to the family’s values and customs,

provided specific information that

the parent needed, and helped

the parent feel like a partner in

their child’s care; this method of

assessing FCC has been used in

multiple studies. FCC is included in

the National Survey of Children’s

Health and the National Survey of

Children with Special Health Care

Needs, 20, 21 and it has been shown

to be stable over multiple waves of

these national surveys. 22 However,

because we cannot know whether

FCC measured at wave 3 is indicative

of care received at waves 1 and 2, a

sensitivity analysis was conducted

to determine whether inclusion of

FCC as a covariate in adjusted models

significantly changed our results.

Statistical Methods

All analyses use design and

nonresponse weights and account

for the effects of weights and

clustering of children within sites by

using Stata SE 10. 23 – 25 Our sample

included 4297 parent–child dyads

that participated in all 3 waves.

We used χ2 tests of homogeneity
and t tests to describe the wave
1 characteristics of children and

parents in the study sample. Bivariate

analyses were also used to describe

the proportion of children with

symptoms consistent with ADHD,

parent-reported diagnosis of ADHD,

and a history of parent-reported

medication for ADHD according to

child race/ethnicity and survey wave.

The proportion of children receiving

ADHD medication was examined

according to race/ethnicity at each

wave, stratified according to number

of ADHD symptoms (0 symptoms,

1–2 symptoms, 3–5 symptoms, and

6–7 symptoms). Logistic regression

3
by guest on October 15, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

COKER et al

was used to examine the unadjusted

and adjusted odds of ADHD diagnosis

and medication use according to

race/ethnicity over the 3 survey

waves. To determine the disparity

in medication use among children

who had received a diagnosis, odds

of medication use were calculated

according to race/ethnicity among

children with a diagnosis of ADHD

(with or without symptoms).

Finally, we looked for racial/ethnic

disparities in medication use among

2 groups of children: (1) those with a

presumed need for ADHD medication

(either an ADHD diagnosis or

symptoms suspicious for ADHD); and

(2) those with no presumed need

for ADHD medication (no diagnosis

of or symptoms consistent with

ADHD by fifth grade). Racial ethnic/

disparities in medication use that

persist in this first group of children

would suggest underdiagnosis or

undertreatment of African-American

and Latino children. Disparities

that persist in the second group of

children would suggest overdiagnosis

or overtreatment of white children,

which could be the result of multiple

factors (eg, differential provider or

parental expectations for medication

use among children based on child

race/ethnicity).

RESULTS

Table 1 describes the sample. In fifth

grade, parents reported that 8% of

children had symptoms of ADHD, 8%

had ever received an ADHD diagnosis,

and 7% had taken medication for

ADHD over the past year. By 10th

grade, those percentages increased

to 9%, 9%, and 8%, respectively. In

fifth, seventh, and 10th grades, higher

percentages of African-American

children compared with white

children had symptoms suggestive

of ADHD (fifth grade, 12% vs 7%;

seventh grade, 11% vs 6%; and 10th

grade, 13% vs 9%). Latino children

were just as likely to have ADHD

symptoms as white children at each

wave ( Table 2).

In fifth and 10th grades, white

children were much more likely

to have ever received a diagnosis

of ADHD (16% in fifth grade and

19% in 10th grade) than African-

American children (9% and 10%,

respectively), Latino children (4%

and 4%), and children of other race/

ethnicity (10% and 10%) ( Tables

2 and 3). White children were also

more likely to have a parental report

of taking medication for ADHD in the

last year at all 3 waves, compared

with African-American, Latino, and

other children ( Table 3). Results for

differences in medication use were

similar when stratified according

to number of ADHD symptoms. At

all symptom levels above zero, a

higher proportion of white children,

compared with African-American

and Latino children, had a parental

report of ADHD medication (see

Supplemental Table 6). This disparity

persisted even among children at

the highest symptom levels. For

example, among 10th grade children

at the highest symptom level, 65%

4

TABLE 1 Fifth Grade Characteristics

Characteristic Unweighted N Weighted % or Mean ± SD

Child race/ethnicity

African-American 1497 29.1

Latino 1512 44.4

Othera 248 4.4

White 1039 22.1

Male sex 2097 51.1

Age (child age at fi fth grade)

≤10 y (most aged 10; n = 16 are 8–9 y) 1989 44.0
11 y 2048 48.9

≥12 y 260 7.1
Highest household education

Some high school 755 23.

5

High school graduate 850 21.

8

Some college 1159 25.2

College graduate 1474 29.5

Household income, $

<20 000 1306 35.4

20 000–34 000 865 23.0

35 000–69 000 857 20.1

≥70 000 1059 21.5
Family household composition

Two-parent 2400 58.1

Single-parent 1685 37.7

Other (nonparent, foster) 190 4.2

Insurance type (child)

Private 2063 42.5

Medicaid/CHIP 1664 42.3

Other insurance type (military, IHS) 80 2.0

Uninsured 472 13.2

Study site

Birmingham, AL 1350 31.0

Houston, TX 1462 34.

6

Los Angeles, CA 1485 34.4

Mental health symptoms

Oppositional defi ant disorder 329 7.8

Conduct disorder 350 8.2

Depression 307 7.5

FCC 2176 48.9

School functioning (PedsQL subscale) — 75.0 ± 20.6

CHIP, Children’s Health Insurance Program; IHS, Indian Health Service; PedsQL, Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory; —,

continuous variable.
a The other category includes multiracial (n = 131), American Indian/Alaska Native (n = 7), and Asian or Pacifi c Islander

(n = 110).

by guest on October 15, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

PEDIATRICS Volume 138 , number 3 , September 2016

of white children were taking ADHD

medication according to parental

report, compared with 36% of

African-American children and 30%

of Latino children.

Across all waves, African-American

children had significantly lower

adjusted odds of both ever having

a diagnosis of ADHD (fifth grade

adjusted odds ratio [aOR], 0.40 [95%

confidence interval (CI), 0.27–0.59];

10th grade aOR, 0.42 [95% CI,

0.27–0.67]) and of taking ADHD

medication in the past year (fifth

grade aOR, 0.43 [95% CI, 0.29–0.65];

seventh grade aOR, 0.41 [95% CI,

0.28–0.62]; 10th grade aOR, 0.44

[95% CI, 0.28–0.71]) compared with

white children. A similar pattern was

observed when comparing Latino

children versus white children

on adjusted odds of ever having a

diagnosis of ADHD (fifth grade aOR,

0.37 [95% CI, 0.22–0.60]; 10th grade

aOR, 0.46 [95% CI, 0.26–0.79]) and

of taking ADHD medication (fifth

grade aOR, 0.40 [95% CI, 0.23–0.70];

seventh grade aOR, 0.43 [95% CI,

0.25–0.74]; 10th grade aOR, 0.41

[95% CI, 0.21–0.79]) ( Table 3). Of

note, male sex was consistently

associated in these models with

receiving an ADHD diagnosis and

medication.

Disparities in Medication Rates
Among Children With ADHD
According to Symptoms or Diagnosis

Among children ever having a

diagnosis of ADHD or past-year

symptoms of ADHD, African-American

children had lower adjusted odds

of past-year ADHD medication,

compared with white children at

fifth grade (aOR, 0.33 [95% CI,

0.17–0.62]), seventh grade (aOR,

0.34 [95% CI, 0.18–0.64]), and 10th

grade (aOR, 0.41 [95% CI, 0.22–0.75]).

Latino children had decreased odds

compared with white children at fifth

grade (aOR, 0.38 [95% CI, 0.16–0.90])

and 10th grade (aOR, 0.42 [95% CI,

0.20-0.86]) only ( Table 4).

When examining disparities in

medication use among children who

had been diagnosed with ADHD

(whether they had symptoms),

African-American children (fifth

grade odds ratio [OR], 0.46 [95% CI,

0.22–0.97]; 10th grade OR, 0.42 [95%

CI, 0.24–0.74]) and Latino children

(fifth grade OR, 0.17 [95% CI, 0.07–

0.39]; 10th grade OR, 0.28 [95% CI,

0.14–0.57]) had lower unadjusted

5

TABLE 2 ADHD Symptoms, Diagnosis, and Medication Use According to Race/Ethnicity Over 3 Waves

Variable Fifth Grade Seventh Grade 10th Grade

ADHD, by symptoms

Total 8 (350) 7 (324) 9 (400)

White 7 (68) 6 (67) 9 (87)

African-American 12 (176) 11 (154) 13 (195)

Latino 6 (90) 6 (89) 6 (95)

Other 7 (16) 6 (14) 9 (23)

P <.001 <.001 <.001

ADHD, by diagnosis

Total 8 (368) NA 9 (422)

White 16 (152) NA 19 (191)

African-American 9 (131) NA 10 (139)

Latino 4 (62) NA 4 (69)

Other 10 (23) NA 10 (23)

P <.001 <.001 Took medication for ADHD (past 12 mo)

Total 7 (314) 7 (336) 8 (341)

White 14 (132) 14 (142) 16 (155)

African-American 9 (123) 9 (124) 8 (110)

Latino 3 (44) 3 (55) 4 (60)

Other 7 (15) 6 (15) 7 (16)

P <.001 <.001 <.001

Unless otherwise indicated, data are presented as n (%). NA, not applicable.

TABLE 3 Unadjusted ORs and aORs of ADHD Diagnosis and Medication Use According to Race/
Ethnicity Over 3 Waves

Variable Fifth Grade Seventh Grade 10th Grade

ADHD, diagnosis

White Ref NA Ref

African-American

OR (95% CI) 0.54 (0.43–0.69)*** NA 0.46 (0.36-0.60)***

aOR (95% CI) 0.40 (0.27–0.59)*** NA 0.42 (0.27–0.67)***

Latino

OR (95% CI) 0.21 (0.15–0.30)*** NA 0.18 (0.13–0.26)***

aOR (95% CI) 0.37 (0.22–0.60)*** NA 0.46 (0.26–0.79)**

Other

OR (95% CI) 0.63 (0.38–1.03) NA 0.45 (0.29–0.72)**

aOR (95% CI) 0.76 (0.40–1.41) NA 0.56 (0.30–1.03)

ADHD, medication

White Ref Ref Ref

African-American

OR (95% CI) 0.57 (0.43–0.75)*** 0.58 (0.45–0.75)*** 0.48 (0.35–0.66)***

aOR (95% CI) 0.43 (0.29–0.65)*** 0.41 (0.28–0.62)*** 0.44 (0.28–0.71)**

Latino

OR (95% CI) 0.18 (0.12–0.25)*** 0.21 (0.15–0.29)*** 0.23 (0.17–0.31)***

aOR (95% CI) 0.40 (0.23–0.70)** 0.43 (0.25–0.74)** 0.41 (0.21–0.79)**

Other

OR (95% CI) 0.45 (0.25–0.80)** 0.39 (0.23–0.67)** 0.42 (0.22–0.79)**

aOR (95% CI) 0.60 (0.31–1.18) 0.46 (0.25–0.86)* 0.45 (0.21–0.98)*

Adjusted for child age, sex, health insurance, mental health symptoms, and school functioning; household composition,

income, and highest parental educational attainment; parent English profi ciency and reported receipt of FCC in child’s

health care; and study site. NA, not applicable.
* P < .05. ** P < .01. *** P < .001.

by guest on October 15, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

COKER et al

odds of medication use compared

with white children. This sample

limited to children with a diagnosis

did not have adequate numbers to

support adjusted analysis (n = 328
for fifth grade; n = 368 for 10th
grade); results not shown in table.

Medication Rates Among Children
Without ADHD According to
Symptoms or Diagnosis by Fifth
Grade

Among children who had neither

past-year symptoms consistent with

ADHD nor an ADHD diagnosis by fifth

grade (and thus would not likely meet

ADHD diagnostic criteria at any age),

there was no statistically significant

difference in past-year medication use

according to race/ethnicity (1%–2%

of white, African-American, and

Latino children were taking ADHD

medication at fifth grade and 2%–3%

at seventh grade). By 10th grade,

the difference in medication use was

statistically significant (P = .004);
7% of white children without ADHD

according to symptoms or diagnosis in

fifth grade took ADHD medication in

the past year, compared with 4% for

African-American children and 3% for

Latino children. In adjusted analyses,

however, the odds of medication use

were not statistically significant for

African-American and Latino children

compared with white children at fifth,

seventh, and 10th grades ( Table 5).

Findings were not sensitive to

inclusion of FCC care as a covariate.

DISCUSSION

In this longitudinal, multisite study,

African-American and Latino

children were less likely to have a

parental report of ever receiving an

ADHD diagnosis or of taking ADHD

medication in the past year compared

with white children; the disparity

in medication use persisted among

children with either a diagnosis of

ADHD or with symptoms of ADHD.

The disparity was not observed in

adjusted analysis among children who

had no history of ADHD according to

diagnosis or symptoms by fifth grade.

Other large studies have found similar

disparities in ADHD diagnosis for

African-American and/or Latino

children, including the 2003–2004 and

2007 National Survey of Children’s

Health, 7, 26, 27 the Early Childhood

Longitudinal Survey–Kindergarten

Cohort, 2, 3 the 1997 to 2001 National

Health Interview Survey, 6 and the 1997

to 2005 Medical Expenditure Panel

survey. 5 Similar disparities have also

been reported in medication use for

ADHD, finding that African-American

and/or Latino children are less likely

to take a medication for ADHD. 1, 3, 5, 6, 27

Reports from 3 waves of the National

Survey of Children’s Health (2003,

2007, and 2011–2012) suggest that

racial/ethnic disparities in diagnosis

and medication treatment continued to

emerge over time, reflecting the trends

of increasing parent-reported ADHD

diagnosis and medication use in the

United States from 2003 to 2011. 1, 27

Not all studies, however, have found

racial/ethnic disparities in ADHD

diagnosis or medication use. 28, 29

For example, Froehlich et al, 29

using 2001–2004 National Health

and Nutrition Examination Survey

data, found no such disparities

among children meeting Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual of Mental

6

TABLE 4 aORs of ADHD Medication Use by Race/Ethnicity Over 3 Waves Among Children With a Diagnosis or Symptoms of ADHD

Child Race/Ethnicity Fifth Grade (n = 577) Seventh Grade (n = 721) 10th Grade (n = 645)

% (N) aOR (95% CI) % (N) aOR (95% CI) % (N) aOR (95% CI)

Total 47 (270) 36 (261) 45 (282)

White 73 (125) Ref 61 (118) Ref 67 (143) Ref

African-American 41 (101) 0.33 (0.17–0.62)a 33 (97) 0.34 (0.18–0.64)a 35 (88) 0.41 (0.22–0.75)a

Latino 24 (31) 0.38 (0.16–0.90)a 19 (35) 0.51 (0.23–1.15) 29 (36) 0.42 (0.20–0.86)a

Other 45 (13) 0.37 (0.12–1.10) 29 (11) 0.24 (0.11–0.56)a 44 (15) 0.33 (0.11–0.96)a

Adjusted for child age, sex, health insurance, mental health symptoms, and school functioning; household composition, income, and highest parental educational attainment; parent

English profi ciency and reported receipt of FCC in child’s health care; and study site. Signifi cant fi ndings are bolded. Fifth grade analysis was limited to children with ADHD symptoms or

diagnosis at wave 1. Seventh grade analysis was limited to children with ADHD symptoms or diagnosis at wave 1 or with symptoms at wave 2 (diagnosis not reported at wave 2). Tenth

grade analysis was limited to children with ADHD symptoms or diagnosis at wave 3.
a Signifi cant fi ndings.

TABLE 5 aORs of ADHD Medication Use According to Race/Ethnicity Over 3 Waves Among Children With No Diagnosis or Symptoms of ADHD at Wave 1

Child Race/Ethnicity Fifth Grade (n = 3628) Seventh Grade (n = 3628) 10th Grade (n = 3596)

% (N) aOR (95% CI) % (N) aOR (95% CI) % (N) aOR (95% CI)

Total 1 (44) 2 (86) 4 (138)

White 1 (7) Ref 3 (25) Ref 7 (56) Ref

African-American 2 (22) 1.03 (0.27–3.96) 3 (33) 0.73 (0.30–1.74) 4 (39) 0.59 (0.28–1.22)

Latino 1 (13) 0.51 (0.12–2.23) 2 (23) 0.85 (0.31–2.34) 3 (38) 0.55 (0.24–1.29)

Other 1 (2) 1.23 (0.13–11.64) 2 (5) 1.08 (0.30–3.92) 4 (5) 0.70 (0.25–2.02)

Adjusted for child age, sex, health insurance, mental health symptoms, and school functioning; household composition, income, and highest parental educational attainment; parent

English profi ciency and reported receipt of FCC in child’s health care; and study site.

by guest on October 15, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

PEDIATRICS Volume 138 , number 3 , September 2016

Disorders, Fourth Edition, criteria
for ADHD. Differences in study

findings may relate to such factors as

relative distribution of white versus

nonwhite participants, or the breadth

of socioeconomic covariates included

in adjusted analyses.

Few previously published studies

have empirically addressed the

question of whether ADHD medication

disparities are primarily due to

underdiagnosis/undertreatment of

African-American and Latino children

or overdiagnosis/overtreatment of

white children. One commentary

addressed the question of

overdiagnosis but without empirical

data. 30 Another study focused on

whether overdiagnosis of ADHD

was a problem, based on previously

published ADHD prevalence data. 8

We found that, among children with a

potential need for ADHD medication

(ie, ADHD symptoms or diagnosis),

African-American and Latino children

were less likely to take ADHD

medication than white children;

however, among children with no

apparent need for ADHD medication

(ie, no ADHD symptoms or diagnosis),

white children were not significantly

more likely to take medication. Our

study does not have an objective

measure of ADHD prevalence, and

thus we are unable to directly address

this question of overdiagnosis and

overtreatment. However, our findings

do indirectly suggest that these

disparities are more likely from the

underdiagnosis/undertreatment of
African-American and Latino children

than the overdiagnosis/overtreatment

of white children. It may be that

African-American and Latino children

are less likely to report taking a

medication for ADHD because they

are less likely to receive a diagnosis

of ADHD, or because when diagnosed,

they are less like to receive (or accept)

a medication for ADHD.

In fifth and seventh grades, only

small percentages of children

with neither symptoms nor a

diagnosis of ADHD by fifth grade

were taking ADHD medication

(across all racial/ethnic groups of

children); the implication of this

finding is that overdiagnosis and

overtreatment likely contribute

only minimally to the disparity

in medication treatment. This

proportion increased for 10th

grade, particularly among white

children, and in unadjusted analysis,

there was a significant difference

between white children compared

with African-American and Latino

children. We found no statistically

significant differences in adjusted

odds, which may be due to the small

number of children with medication

use in those models.

This study has limitations. First,

parent-reported data may introduce

reporting bias; however, a recent

study reported similarities between

parent-reported ADHD estimates and

administrative claims data estimates. 31

In addition, we were unable to include

reports from teachers or schools

on school functioning, which is an

important element of ADHD symptom

reporting. Our child-reported school

functioning measures are limited

in that they focused on behavior,

were not validated against teacher

ratings, and may not accurately reflect

academic achievement. It is also

possible that there are racial/ethnic

differences in parental reporting of

symptoms for which we are not able to

account; these differences in reporting

could potentially lead to either an

underestimation or overestimation

of the disparities in our findings. Data

collection was limited to children in

public school settings in 3 metropolitan

areas, and our sample’s racial/

ethnic composition is different from

the US composition because it was

designed to have a balanced sample of

African-American, Latino, and white

children; thus, caution should be used

in generalizing our findings to other

populations. We also lacked detailed

information on ADHD medication

use (eg, type and dose of medication,

duration of use); these data should be

considered for future studies.

CONCLUSIONS

Our findings have implications for the

diagnosis and treatment of ADHD.

There are various improvements in

care that may help in closing this gap in

diagnosis and treatment. These include

actively and universally eliciting

parental concerns about child behavior

and academic performance (at home

and school) at well-visits, 32, 33 providing

care that is culturally relevant in

families’ preferred languages, 34 and

linking with community resources

to provide mental health education,

guidance, and services to families (eg,

parent training courses for parents of

children with ADHD). 35 – 39 Pediatric

clinicians also may need to consider

universal behavioral health screening

tools for children to improve diagnostic

capabilities and recognize when a

child has ADHD symptoms, even if

the problem is not recognized by the

parent.

Because the rates of diagnosis

and treatment are rising in the

general population of US children, a

significant need remains to identify

and treat African-American and

Latino children who have ADHD and

avoid a widening of these disparities.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank the Healthy Passages team,

staff, and participants for making the

study possible.

7

ABBREVIATIONS

aOR:  adjusted odds ratio

ADHD:  attention-deficit/

hyperactivity disorder

CI:  confidence interval

DPS:  Diagnostic Interview

Schedule for Children

Predictive Scales

OR:  odds ratio

by guest on October 15, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

COKER et al

REFERENCES

1. Visser SN, Danielson ML, Bitsko RH,

et al. Trends in the parent-report

of health care provider-diagnosed

and medicated attention-defi cit/

hyperactivity disorder: United States,

2003-2011. J Am Acad Child Adolesc

Psychiatry. 2014;53(1):34–46.e2

2. Schneider H, Eisenberg D. Who receives

a diagnosis of attention-defi cit/

hyperactivity disorder in the United

States elementary school population?

Pediatrics. 2006;117(4). Available at:

www. pediatrics. org/ cgi/ content/ full/

117/ 4/ e601

3. Morgan PL, Staff J, Hillemeier MM,

Farkas G, Maczuga S. Racial and

ethnic disparities in ADHD diagnosis

from kindergarten to eighth grade.

Pediatrics. 2013;132(1):85–93

4. Mehta NK, Lee H, Ylitalo KR. Child health

in the United States: recent trends in

racial/ethnic disparities. Soc Sci Med.

2013;95:6–15

5. Stevens J, Harman JS, Kelleher KJ.

Race/ethnicity and insurance status

as factors associated with ADHD

treatment patterns. J Child Adolesc

Psychopharmacol. 2005;15(1):88–96

6. Pastor PN, Reuben CA. Racial and

ethnic differences in ADHD and LD in

young school-age children: parental

reports in the National Health

Interview Survey. Public Health Rep.

2005;120(4):383–392

7. Flores G, Tomany-Korman SC. Racial

and ethnic disparities in medical and

dental health, access to care, and use

of services in US children. Pediatrics.

2008;121(2). Available at: www.

pediatrics. org/ cgi/ content/ full/ 121/ 2/

e286

8. Sciutto MJ, Eisenberg M. Evaluating

the evidence for and against the

overdiagnosis of ADHD. J Atten Disord.

2007;11(2):106–113

9. Windle M, Grunbaum JA, Elliott M,

et al. Healthy passages. A multilevel,

multimethod longitudinal study of

adolescent health. Am J Prev Med.

2004;27(2):164–172

10. Schuster MA, Elliott MN, Kanouse DE,

et al. Racial and ethnic health disparities

among fi fth-graders in three cities.

N Engl J Med. 2012;367(8):735–745

11. Lucas CP, Zhang H, Fisher PW, et al.

The DISC Predictive Scales (DPS):

effi ciently screening for diagnoses.

J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry.

2001;40(4):443–449

12. American Psychiatric Association.

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of

Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington,

DC: American Psychiatric Association;

2013

13. Kataoka SH, Zhang L, Wells KB. Unmet

need for mental health care among

U.S. children: variation by ethnicity

and insurance status. Am J Psychiatry.

2002;159(9):1548–1555

14. Cunningham PJ, Freiman MP.

Determinants of ambulatory mental

health services use for school-age

children and adolescents. Health Serv

Res. 1996;31(4):409–427

15. Zimmerman FJ. Social and economic

determinants of disparities in

professional help-seeking for child

mental health problems: evidence from

a national sample. Health Serv Res.

2005;40(5 pt 1):1514–1533

16. Varni JW, Burwinkle TM, Seid M,

Skarr D. The PedsQL 4.0 as a pediatric

population health measure: feasibility,

reliability, and validity. Ambul Pediatr.

2003;3(6):329–341

17. Strickland BB, Jones JR, Ghandour RM,

Kogan MD, Newacheck PW. The medical

home: health care access and impact

for children and youth in the United

States. Pediatrics. 2011;127(4):604–611

18. Ngui EM, Flores G. Satisfaction

with care and ease of using health

care services among parents of

children with special health care

needs: the roles of race/ethnicity,

insurance, language, and adequacy

of family-centered care. Pediatrics.

2006;117(4):1184–1196

19. Coker TR, Rodriguez MA, Flores G.

Family-centered care for US children

with special health care needs:

who gets it and why? Pediatrics.

2010;125(6):1159–1167

20. Child and Adolescent Health

Measurement Initiative. National

Survey of Children’s Health. Available

at: www. childhealthdata. org. Accessed

October 15, 2015

21. Child and Adolescent Health

Measurement Initiative. National

Survey of Children with

Special Health Care Needs.

8

obtained funding, participated in study design and interpretation of fi ndings, revised manuscript drafts, and contributed to overall supervision; and all authors

provided fi nal approval for the submitted manuscript.

DOI: 10.1542/peds.2016-0407

Accepted for publication Jun 30,

2016

Address correspondence to Tumaini R. Coker, MD, MBA, Mattel Children’s Hospital, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, 10833 Le Conte Ave, Room 12-436, Los

Angeles, CA 90095. E-mail: tcoker@mednet.ucla.edu

PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275).

Copyright © 2016 by the American Academy of Pediatrics

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no fi nancial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

FUNDING: The Healthy Passages Study was funded by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Prevention Research Centers (Cooperative Agreements
CCU409679, CCU609653, CCU915773, U48DP000046, U48DP000057, U48DP000056, U19DP002663, U19DP002664, and U19DP002665). The fi ndings and conclusions in

this report are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the offi cial position of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.

POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential confl icts of interest to disclose.

by guest on October 15, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

PEDIATRICS Volume 138 , number 3 , September 2016

Available at: www. childhealthdata. org.

Accessed October 15, 2015

22. National Survey of Children’s Health

and National Survey of Children with

Special Health Care Needs. Data query

from the Child and Adolescent Health

Measurement Initiative, Data Resource

Center for Child and Adolescent

Health Web site. Available at: www.

childhealthdata. org. Accessed April 6,

2016

23. Williams RL. A note on robust variance

estimation for cluster-correlated data.

Biometrics. 2000;56(2):645–646

24. Skinner CJ. Domain means, regression

and multivariate analyses. In: Skinner

CJ, Holt D, Smith TMF, eds. Analysis of

Complex Surveys. Chichester, UK: Wiley;

1989:59–88

25. Wooldridge J. Econometric Analysis

of Cross Section and Panel Data.

Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2002

26. Flores G, Lin H. Trends in racial/ethnic

disparities in medical and oral health,

access to care, and use of services

in US children: has anything changed

over the years? Int J Equity Health.

2013;12(10):10

27. Centers for Disease Control and

Prevention (CDC). Increasing

prevalence of parent-reported

attention-defi cit/hyperactivity

disorder among children—United

States, 2003 and 2007. MMWR Morb

Mortal Wkly Rep. 2010;59(44):

1439–1443

28. Visser SN, Lesesne CA, Perou R.

National estimates and factors

associated with medication treatment

for childhood attention-defi cit/

hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics.

2007;119(suppl 1):S99–S106

29. Froehlich TE, Lanphear BP, Epstein JN,

Barbaresi WJ, Katusic SK, Kahn RS.

Prevalence, recognition, and treatment

of attention-defi cit/hyperactivity

disorder in a national sample of US

children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med.

2007;161(9):857–864

30. Morley CP. Disparities in ADHD

assessment, diagnosis, and

treatment. Int J Psychiatry Med.

2010;40(4):383–389

31. Visser SN, Danielson ML, Bitsko RH,

Perou R, Blumberg SJ. Convergent

validity of parent-reported attention-

defi cit/hyperactivity disorder

diagnosis: a cross-study comparison.

JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167(7):674–675

32. Coker TR, Shaikh Y, Chung PJ.

Parent-reported quality of preventive

care for children at-risk for

developmental delay. Acad Pediatr.

2012;12(5):384–390

33. Guerrero AD, Rodriguez MA, Flores

G. Disparities in provider elicitation

of parents’ developmental concerns

for US children. Pediatrics.

2011;128(5):901–909

34. Zuckerman KE, Mattox KM, Sinche BK,

Blaschke GS, Bethell C. Racial, ethnic,

and language disparities in early

childhood developmental/behavioral

evaluations: a narrative review. Clin

Pediatr (Phila). 2014;53(7):619–631

35. Perrin EC, Sheldrick RC, McMenamy

JM, Henson BS, Carter AS. Improving

parenting skills for families of young

children in pediatric settings: a

randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr.

2014;168(1):16–24

36. Zwi M, Jones H, Thorgaard C,

York A, Dennis JA. Parent training

interventions for Attention Defi cit

Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

in children aged 5 to 18 years.

Cochrane Database Syst Rev.

2011;12(12):CD003018

37. Fabiano GA, Chacko A, Pelham WE

Jr, et al. A comparison of behavioral

parent training programs for fathers

of children with attention-defi cit/

hyperactivity disorder. Behav Ther.

2009;40(2):190–204

38. Chronis-Tuscano A, O’Brien KA,

Johnston C, et al. The relation

between maternal ADHD symptoms

& improvement in child behavior

following brief behavioral parent

training is mediated by change in

negative parenting. J Abnorm Child

Psychol. 2011;39(7):1047–1057

39. Loren RE, Vaughn AJ, Langberg JM,

et al Effects of an 8-session behavioral

parent training group for parents

of children with ADHD on child

impairment and parenting confi dence.

J Atten Disord. 2015;19(2):158–166

9
by guest on October 15, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

DOI: 10.1542/peds.2016-0407 originally published online August 23, 2016;
2016;138;Pediatrics

Schuster
Cuccaro, Susan Tortolero Emery, Susan L. Davies, Susanna N. Visser and Mark A.

Tumaini R. Coker, Marc N. Elliott, Sara L. Toomey, David C. Schwebel, Paula
Racial and Ethnic Disparities in ADHD Diagnosis and Treatment

Services
Updated Information &

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/138/3/e20160407
including high resolution figures, can be found at:

References
http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/138/3/e20160407#BIBL
This article cites 32 articles, 8 of which you can access for free at:

Subspecialty Collections

activity_disorder_adhd_sub
http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/attention-deficit:hyper
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
al_issues_sub
http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/development:behavior
Developmental/Behavioral Pediatrics
following collection(s):
This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in the

Permissions & Licensing

http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml
in its entirety can be found online at:
Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or

Reprints
http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml
Information about ordering reprints can be found online:

by guest on October 15, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

http://http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/138/3/e20160407

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/138/3/e20160407#BIBL

http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/development:behavioral_issues_sub

http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/development:behavioral_issues_sub

http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/attention-deficit:hyperactivity_disorder_adhd_sub

http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/attention-deficit:hyperactivity_disorder_adhd_sub

http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xhtml

http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

DOI: 10.1542/peds.2016-0407 originally published online August 23, 2016;
2016;138;Pediatrics
Schuster
Cuccaro, Susan Tortolero Emery, Susan L. Davies, Susanna N. Visser and Mark A.
Tumaini R. Coker, Marc N. Elliott, Sara L. Toomey, David C. Schwebel, Paula
Racial and Ethnic Disparities in ADHD Diagnosis and Treatment

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/138/3/e20160407
located on the World Wide Web at:

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/suppl/2016/08/22/peds.2016-0407.DCSupplemental
Data Supplement at:

by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 1073-0397.
the American Academy of Pediatrics, 345 Park Avenue, Itasca, Illinois, 60143. Copyright © 2016
has been published continuously since 1948. Pediatrics is owned, published, and trademarked by
Pediatrics is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it

by guest on October 15, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/138/3/e20160407

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/suppl/2016/08/22/peds.2016-0407.DCSupplemental

O R I G I N A L A R T I C L E

Is ADHD a ‘real’ disorder?

MICHAEL QUINN and ANDREA LYNCH

In many western countries, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
has achieved celebrity status, such that it probably no longer requires intro-
duction. The disorder is a global phenomenon, spreading rapidly as result of
the increasing dominance internationally of US psychiatric models, the need
for new markets for major pharmaceutical companies, increasing use of the
internet by parents and professionals and changing approaches to schooling.
There is a broad consensus among international experts and organisations
that ADHD is a genuine neurodevelopmental disorder based on empirical
research. However, many critics have questioned the legitimacy of ADHD.
This paper reviews the arguments for and against the ADHD construct. First,
the review examines the literature and research endorsing ADHD as a ‘real’
disorder. Second, the criticisms levelled against the ADHD construct are
examined.

Key words: ADHD, support, science, criticisms.

Introduction

According to the American Psychiatric Association (2013), ADHD is a neurode-

velopmental disorder characterised by a persistent pattern of inattention and

hyperactivity–impulsivity. Symptoms of this nature persist into adulthood and

can cause several impairments in social, academic and occupational functioning

(Gapin et al., 2011). Globally, the disorder affects 5.3% of children (Polanczyk
et al., 2007). This figure was reported following a review of 102 prevalence

VC 2016 NASEN
DOI: 10.1111/1467-9604.12114

studies from seven regions, including North America, South America, Europe,

Africa, the Middle East, Asia and Oceania. The disorder is more commonly

diagnosed in males than in females in the general population, with a ratio of 2:1

in children (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In the United States, and

increasingly in Europe, psychostimulants are first-line treatments for ADHD

(Singh, 2008) and since the 1950s, medications for ADHD have been used

(Kewley, 2011).

In many western countries, ADHD has achieved celebrity status, such that it

probably no longer requires introduction (Graham, 2010). It is a global phenom-

enon, spreading rapidly as result of the increasing dominance internationally of

US psychiatric models, the need for new markets for major pharmaceutical com-

panies, increasing use of the internet by parents and professionals and changing

approaches to schooling (Stead et al., 2006). Campbell (2000) asserts it is safe
to argue that we likely know more about ADHD than any other childhood con-

dition. Yet, despite the existing plethora of research and skyrocketing increases

in the number of persons diagnosed with ADHD, it is clear that this topic is

highly misunderstood (Kewley, 1999).

Support for ADHD

The symptoms of ADHD do not represent a new phenomenon (Goldstein and

Goldstein, 1998): the British physician George Still made reference to a disorder

we now recognise as ADHD in 1902 (Cooper and Bilton, 2002). Descriptions of

inattention, impulsive and hyperactive behaviour in childhood have appeared in

texts as old as the Bible (Goldstein and Goldstein, 1998). Many international

experts and organisations have endorsed ADHD as a valid medical disorder (e.g.

Barkley, 2013; DuPaul and Stoner, 2014; Mash and Wolfe, 2015; Tannock,

1998; Kewley, 2011; Goldstein and Goldstein, 1998; American Psychiatric

Association, 2013; National Institute of Mental Health, 2008; British Psycholog-

ical Society, 2000; National Institute of Clinical Excellence, 2013). Many of

these organisations, among others, have published comprehensive guidelines

that provide evidence-based recommendations for the diagnosis and treatment of

ADHD. In 2002, Professor Russell Barkley and a consortium of medical practi-

tioners and researchers published an International Consensus Statement on
ADHD (Barkley et al., 2002). This document is significant in the context of the
current article because it confirmed the status of the scientific findings

60 Support for Learning � Volume 31 � Number 1 � 2016 VC 2016 NASEN

concerning the validity of the disorder, and its adverse impact on the lives of

those living with ADHD.

Science and ADHD

Findings from genetic and neurological studies have given weight to the argu-

ment that ADHD is a valid disorder. For example, Tannock (1998) reviewed

several studies and identified the following findings which support a genetic

basis for ADHD:

� Over the past 30 years, numerous family-genetic studies have reported a
higher prevalence rate of psychopathology, particularly ADHD, in parents

and other relatives of children with ADHD.

� In twin and adoption studies, reports have consistently shown a much
greater incidence of ADHD among identical monozygotic (MZ) twins than

among non-identical dizygotic (DZ) twins. Passmore (2014) also adds that

MZ twins share 100 per cent of their genes, whereas DZ twins only share

50 per cent of their genes: ‘Scientists have found that if one twin has

symptoms of ADHD, the risk that the other will have the disorder is as

high as 75-90%’ (Barkley, 2013).

� In the dopamine system, molecular genetic research has identified genetic
abnormalities. Dopamine is one of many neurotransmitters found in the

brain and is essential for attention among other things (Ratey and Hager-

man, 2008).

There have also been suggestions that ADHD may have its roots in neurological

causes (Buckley et al., 2009; Barkley and Murphy, 2006; Jacobs and Wendel,
2010). Neurotransmitter chemicals such as dopamine, norepinephrine and sero-

tonin play a vital role in regulating human behaviour (Reynolds et al., 2012;
Parker, 1998). These neurotransmitters carry messages between brain cells dur-

ing mental tasks – rather like workers moving around and putting things together

in a factory (Munden and Arcelus, 1999). Ratey and Hagerman (2008) explain

why these particular neurotransmitters are so important in the case of children

with ADHD. First, the neurotransmitter dopamine is essential for attention,

among other things. Second, norepinephrine affects arousal, alertness, attention

and mood. Third, serotonin regulates many functions, including mood, impulsiv-

ity, learning and self-esteem. Professionals maintain that children with ADHD

have a deficiency in these neurotransmitter chemicals (Barkley, 2013; Ratey and

VC 2016 NASEN Support for Learning � Volume 31 � Number 1 � 2016 61

Hagerman, 2008; Reiff and Tippins, 2004). Studies have discovered particularly

low levels of activity in the neurotransmitters located in the frontal lobes of the

brain (Wheeler, 2007). Findings from various studies support the argument that

neurological factors are a key contributor to ADHD. Neuro-imaging studies

involving children with ADHD have shown a decreased size of the prefrontal

cortex (e.g. Mostofsky et al., 2002), resulting in expected deficits in certain pre-
frontal executive functions, such as response inhibition and working memory

(Barkley, 1997; Tannock, 1998). In the United States, one study that deserves

particular attention was carried out at the National Institute of Mental Health by

Zametkin et al. (1990). This study has been described as a landmark piece of
research (Ratey and Hagerman, 2008). Using a type of brain scan referred to as

Positron Emission Tomography (PET) scans, the study focused on the rate at

which the brain uses glucose, which is the brain’s main energy source. Results

illustrated that during an attention test, participants with ADHD displayed 10

per cent less brain activity than the control group. The largest deficit was within

the prefrontal cortex, an area of the brain which plays a crucial role in regulating

behaviour, and is also prone to positive reinforcement through physical activity

(Ratey and Hagerman, 2008).

Medicalising ‘annoying’ behaviour

Some critics of the ADHD construct question the possibility that ADHD is per-

haps nothing more than an example of the ‘medicalisation’ of behaviours in

children which are the most annoying and problematic for adults to control. As

Bromfield proclaims, the condition is implicated in ‘all sorts of abuses, hypoc-

risies, neglects, and other society ills that have nothing to do with ADHD’

(Bromfield, 1996, p. 3; cited in Conrad and Potter, 2000, p. 570). Indeed, those

who are critical of ADHD as a medicalised construct often cite ADHD as ‘a

means of labelling and controlling children who exhibit difficult behaviours’

(Mather, 2012, p. 19). Child neurologist Fred Baughman has been one of the

most outspoken critics of ADHD, calling it a ‘fraud’. As Baughman (2006)

argues:

‘Virtually all the symptoms of ADHD relate to classroom behaviour. Children

who don’t do homework, fidget, squirm, interrupt, and are forgetful and disor-

ganized are assumed to have a biochemical imbalance in their brain. These chil-

dren can be difficult to control in a classroom and in many cases are more

62 Support for Learning � Volume 31 � Number 1 � 2016 VC 2016 NASEN

compliant when drugged. However, there is absolutely no scientifically valid

evidence that compliant –drugged students learn faster.’ (2006, p. xiii)

He goes on to conclude: ‘ADHD is a disorder manufactured to match our times.

It is a quick catch-all diagnosis with a magic bullet treatment’ (2006, p. xiii).

Other critics view ADHD as a social construct which is the result of our

performance-driven cultures and societies, citing the prolific increase of Ritalin

and other stimulants to ‘treat’ ADHD as nothing more than a method of perform-

ance enhancement. According to Lawrence Diller (1998), the ADHD label is ‘sal-

vation’ for some, allowing them to avoid feelings of ‘failure’ as they blame their

behaviour and related issues on brain functioning or genetics, rather than accepting

personal responsibility for their problems. The same author contends that the

increased numbers of those diagnosed with ADHD has led to the development of

supportive communities which provide a sense of belonging to people whose

behaviour has otherwise made them outsiders in the dominant culture.

Disagreement on how to ‘define’ ADHD

A review of literature shows there is no consensus on an agreed definition for the

disorder. For the example, in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Dis-
orders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5) published by the American Psychiatric Association
(2013), ADHD is defined as a ‘neurodevelopmental disorder’, and the Interna-
tional Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems refers to it as a
‘hyperkinetic disorder’ (World Health Organization, 1992). ADHD is also defined

in a wide variety of ways within the research literature, and each definition can

give some insight into the author’s philosophical position concerning the nature of

ADHD. It is not uncommon to see ADHD defined as a neurobiologic condition
(Quinn, 2008), a neurodevelopmental disorder (Mrug et al., 2012), a mental disor-
der (Benkert et al., 2010) and a heterogeneous condition (Newcorn et al., 2001;
Faraone and Biederman, 1998). In addition, the following terms have also been

historically applied to ADHD: ‘attention deficit disorder (ADD), hyperkinetic dis-

order (HKD), hyperkinesis, minimal brain dysfunction, minimal brain damage

(MBD), and disorder of attention, motor control, and perception (DAMP)’ (Carr,

2006, p. 421). Regardless of the kind of term used, children with ADHD continue

to present with severe and pervasive symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity and

impulsivity.

VC 2016 NASEN Support for Learning � Volume 31 � Number 1 � 2016 63

Lack of a biological marker and unknown etiology

Despite copious amounts of medical and psychological research, science has yet

to discover a biological marker or characteristic for ADHD. Unlike other medi-

cal conditions which have clear and identifiable markers that indicate the pres-

ence of disorder, in the case of ADHD, ‘No biological marker is diagnostic’

(American Psychiatric Association, 2013, p. 61). Also, there are no objective

medical ‘tests’ which can detect the presence of ADHD (Timimi and Taylor,

2004). Science remains unable to identify the exact causes or etiology of

ADHD, and prominent researchers like Faraone and Biederman admit that we

have yet to achieve this goal (Faraone et al., 1995). The inability to conclusively
identify causes of ADHD calls into serious question the legitimacy of ADHD as

a medical disorder (Visser and Jehan, 2009). Not surprisingly, several theories

exist concerning the causes of ADHD in the individual.

The biological theory of impaired brain functioning is perhaps the most widely

accepted of all theories (Tidefors and Strand, 2012), as it suggests that ADHD is

the result of malfunction in brain processes and/or structures (Qiu et al., 2011;
Castellanos et al., 2002; Taylor, 1999). Genetic factors have also been strongly
linked to ADHD causation (Tannock, 1998; Hawi et al., 2013; Park et al., 2010;
Thapar et al., 2007; Faraone and Biederman, 1998), as have ‘deficits’ in the
individual, such as deficits in executive functioning (Shoemaker et al., 2012), in
executive inhibition (Nigg, 2001) and in behavioural inhibition (Barkley, 1997).

ADHD has also been linked to a host of sources outside of the individual, such

as conditions before, during and after gestation, psychosocial influences and

environmental factors (Thapar et al., 2013; Taylor and Sonuga-Barke, 2008).
The existing body of research is not without its critics. Researchers such as

Ongel (2006) take a more sceptical view of such studies, warning us that associ-
ation between variables (such as abnormal behaviour and brain abnormalities)
does not automatically signify causation.

Diagnosis of ADHD: subjectivity and checklists

The subjective nature of the ADHD diagnosis is another cause for criticism.

Senior (2009) argues that ADHD is so contested precisely because of the subjec-
tive nature of the diagnostic process. ADHD diagnosis is based largely on the

characteristic criteria established in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Associa-
tion, 2013) and the International Classification of Diseases and Related Health

64 Support for Learning � Volume 31 � Number 1 � 2016 VC 2016 NASEN

Problems (World Health Organization, 1992), and is subjectively identified by
medical professionals through use of behavioural checklists and rating scales

(Stead et al., 2006). There is no one standardised checklist in use for ADHD
diagnosis; rather, there are at least two dozen, if not more (Cohen, 2006). Often,

whether a person obtains a diagnosis of ADHD can be a matter of degree and

personal opinion.

Over-diagnosis and use of psychotropic drugs in children

Critics of the ADHD construct also question the ever increasing diagnosis of

ADHD along with skyrocketing rates of psychostimulant drug usage in children.

It is estimated that in the UK during the ten-year period from 1994 to 2004, the

prescription of methylphenidate increased by an astonishing 7,600% (Cohen,

2006). As Cormier (2008) explains, we should be extremely concerned regard-

ing the ever increasing numbers of pre-schoolers who are diagnosed with

ADHD, as well as the ‘sevenfold’ increase in the prescription and use of stimu-

lant medications in children. Statistics such as this raise questions regarding the

possibility of the ‘over-diagnosis’ of ADHD. In their research, Bruchmuller

et al. (2012, p.128) determined that therapists commonly fail to diagnose in
strict accordance with manuals and that ‘overdiagnosis of ADHD occurs in the

clinical routine’. However, others argue against the concept of overdiagnosis,

claiming this is a misperception which has simply taken hold in public percep-

tion and media coverage (Sciutto and Eisenberg, 2007).

Questions have also been raised regarding the influence and motives of the phar-

maceutical industry in the race to ADHD diagnosis (Conrad and Bergey, 2014;

Ongel, 2006). While much of the research literature strongly asserts the efficacy

of psychostimulant drugs in treating and controlling symptoms associated with

ADHD (Benkert et al., 2010; Forness et al., 1999), questions must be raised
regarding the potential for bias, especially in cases where pharmaceutical com-

panies are backing research into the efficacy of pharmacological treatments for

ADHD. One example of such research is the Survey of ADHD in Irish Children
(Fitzgerald, 2007) which was directly supported by the pharmaceutical company

Eli Lilly and Co. (Ireland) Ltd. Interestingly, many of the questions in this ‘sur-

vey’ of parents of children with ADHD highlight strongly positive outcomes

reported by parents when their children were on medication, as opposed to

when they were not.

VC 2016 NASEN Support for Learning � Volume 31 � Number 1 � 2016 65

Conclusion

The authors of this paper reviewed the literature and research supporting ADHD

as a valid disorder. They also explored the many criticisms levelled against the

ADHD construct. Research literature on both sides of the ADHD argument

presents clear and compelling evidence for their theories and positions and it is

clear that the debate surrounding the legitimacy of ADHD as a medical condi-

tion is far from over. Although the ADHD construct has been scientifically

explored, it has gathered limited medical support, and admittedly, serious gaps

in knowledge remain unanswered. This highlights the need for further research

and exploration of this controversial and contested condition, with which many

of our children and students continue to be diagnosed and labelled in the present

day.

References

AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC ASSOCIATION (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of

Mental Disorders (DSM-5) (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
BARKLEY, R. (2013) Taking Charge of ADHD: The Complete Authoritative Guide for Parents.

New York: Guilford Publications, Inc.

BARKLEY, R. et al. (2002) International consensus statement on ADHD. Clinical Child and

Family Psychology Review, 5, 2, 89–111.
BARKLEY, R. (1997) Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions:

constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 1, 65–94.
BARKLEY, R. and MURPHY, K. (2006) Attention-deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Clinical

Workbook (3rd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.

BAUGHMAN, F. (2006) The ADHD Fraud: How Psychiatry makes ‘Patients’ of Normal

Children. Victoria, BC: Trafford Publishing.
BENKERT, D., HENNING-KRAUSE, K., WASEM, J. and AIDELSBURGER, P. (2010)

Effectiveness of pharmaceutical therapy of ADHD (Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder) in

adults—health technology assessment. GMS Health Technology Assessment, 6, 1–12.
BRUCHMULLER, K., MARGRAF, J. and SCHNEIDER, S. (2012) Is ADHD diagnosed in

accord with diagnostic criteria? Overdiagnosis and influence of client gender on diagnosis.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80, 1, 128–138.

BUCKLEY, S., GAVIN, B. and MCNICHOLAS, F. (2009) Mental Health in Children and

Adolescents: A Guide for Teachers. Drogheda, Ireland: Mulberry Publications.
CAMPBELL, S. (2000) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a developmental view. In

Sameroff, A., Lewis, M. and Miller, S. (eds), Handbook of Developmental Psychopathology

(2nd ed.), pp. 383–401. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.

CARR, A. (2006) Attention and over-activity problems. In Carr, A. (ed), The Handbook of Child

and Adolescent Clinical Psychology: A Contextual Approach (2nd ed.), pp. 421–460. London:

Routledge.

66 Support for Learning � Volume 31 � Number 1 � 2016 VC 2016 NASEN

CASTELLANOS, F., LEE, P., SHARP, W., JEFFRIES, N., GREENSTEIN, D., CLASEN, L.,

BLUMENTHAL, J., JAMES, R., EBENS, C., WALTER, J., ZIJDENBOS, A., EVANS, A.,

GIEDD, J. and RAPOPORT, J. (2002) Developmental trajectories of brain volume

abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal

of the American Medical Association, 288, 14, 1740–1748.

COHEN, D. (2006) Critiques of the ADHD enterprise. In Lloyd, G., Stead, J. and Cohen, D.

(eds.) Critical new perspectives on ADHD, pp. 12–44. London: Routledge.

CONRAD, P. and BERGEY, M. (2014) The impending globalization of ADHD: notes on the

expansion and growth of a medicalized disorder. Social Science & Medicine, 122, 31–43.

CONRAD, P. and POTTER, D. (2000) From hyperactive children to ADHD adults: observations

on the expansion of medical categories. Social Problems, 47, 4, 559–582.
COOPER, P. and BILTON, K. (2002) Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Practical

Guide for Teachers (2nd ed.) London: David Fulton.
CORMIER, E. (2008) Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a review and update. Journal of

Pediatric Nursing, 23, 5, 345– 357.
DILLER, L. (1998) Running on Ritalin: A Physician Reflects on Children, Society, and

Performance in a Pill. New York: Bantam Books.
DUPAUL, G. J. and STONER, G. (2014) ADHD in Schools: Assessment and Intervention

Strategies (3rd ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.
FARAONE, S. and BIEDERMAN, J. (1998) Neurobiology of attention-deficit hyperactivity

disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 10, 951–958.
FARAONE, S., BIEDERMAN, J., CHEN, W., MILBERGER, S., WARBURTON, R. and

TSUANG, M. (1995) Genetic heterogeneity in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

(ADHD): gender, psychiatric comorbidity, and maternal ADHD. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 104, 2, 234–345.
FITZGERALD, M. (2007) Survey of ADHD in Irish Children. [Online at http://positivecare.ie/

wp-content/uploads/2012/05/adhd-survey-ireland-parents ]. Accessed 16/04/15.

FORNESS, S., KAVALE, K. and CRENSHAW, T. (1999) Stimulant medication revisited:

effective treatment of children with ADHD. Reclaiming Child Youth, 7, 4, 230–233.

GAPIN, J. L., LABBAN, J. D. and ETNIER, J. L. (2011) The effects of physical activity on attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms: the evidence. Preventive Medicine, 52, 70–74.

GOLDSTEIN, S. and GOLDSTEIN, M. (1998) Managing Attention Deficit Hyperactivity

Disorder in Children (2nd ed.). New York: John Wiley and Sons.

GRAHAM, L. J. (2010) (De)constructing ADHD: Critical Guidance for Teachers and Teacher

Educators. New York: Peter Lang.

HAWI, Z., MATTHEWS, N., WAGNER, J., WALLACE, R., BUTLER, T., VANCE, A., KENT,

L., GILL, M. and BELLGROVE, M. (2013) DNA variation in the SNAP25 gene confers risk

to ADHD and is associated with reduced expression in prefrontal cortex. PloS ONE, 8, 4.
JACOBS, C. and WENDEL, I. (2010) The Everything Parent’s Guide to ADHD in Children.

Avon, MA, U.S. Adams Media.

KENDALL, J., HATTON, D., BECKETT, A. and LEO, M. (2003) Children’s accounts of

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Advances in Nursing Science, 26, 2, 114–130.
KEWLEY, G. (1999) Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Recognition, Reality, snd

Resolution.

London: David Fulton Publishers.

KEWLEY, G. (2011) Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: What can Teachers Do? (3rd ed.).

London: David Fulton Publishers.

VC 2016 NASEN Support for Learning � Volume 31 � Number 1 � 2016 67

http://positivecare.ie/wp-content/uploads/2012/05/adhd-survey-ireland-parents

http://positivecare.ie/wp-content/uploads/2012/05/adhd-survey-ireland-parents

MASH, E. J. and WOLFE, D. A. (2015) Abnormal Child Psychology (6th ed.). Boston, MA:
Cengage Learning.

MATHER, B. (2012) The social construction and reframing of attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder. Ethical Human Psychology & Psychiatry, 14, 1, 15–26.
MRUG, S., MOLINA, B., HOZA, B., GERDES, A., HINSHAW, S., HECHTMAN, L. and

ARNOLD, L. (2012) Peer rejection and friendship in children with attention-deficit/

hyperactivity disorder: contributions to long-term outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology, 40, 6, 1013–1026.
MUNDEN, A. and ARCELUS, J. (1999) The ADHD Handbook: A Guide for Parents and

Professionals on Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

MOSTOFSKY, S. H., COOPER, K. L., KATES, W. R., DENCKLA, M. B. and KAUFMANN,

W. E. (2002) Smaller prefrontal and premotor volumes in boys with attention deficit/

hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 8, 785–794.

NATIONAL INSTITUTE OF MENTAL HEALTH (2008) Attention Deficit Hyperactivity
Disorder (ADHD). Bethesda, MD: National Institute of Mental Health.

NATIONAL INSTITUTE FOR HEALTH AND CLINICAL EXCELLENCE (2013) Attention
Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Diagnosis and Management of ADHD in Children, Young
People and Adults. London: National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence.

NEWCORN, J., HALPERIN, J., JENSEN, P., ABIKOFF, H., ARNOLD, E., CANTWELL, D.,

CONNERS, C., ELLIOTT, G., EPSTEIN, J., GREENHILL, L., HECHTMAN, L.,

HINSHAW, S., HOZA, B., KRAEMER, H., PELHAM, W., SEVERE, J., SWANSON, J.,

WELLS, K., WIGAL, T. and VITIELLO, B. (2001) Symptom profiles in children with

ADHD: effects of comorbidity and gender. Journal of American Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry, 40, 2, 137–146.
NIGG, J. (2001) Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 5, 571–598.

ONGEL, U. (2006) ADHD and parenting styles. In G. Lloyd, J. Stead and D. Cohen, Critical
New Perspectives on ADHD, pp. 115–127. London: Routledge.

PARK, J., WILLMOTT, M., VETUZ, G., TOYE, C., KIRLEY, A., HAWI, Z., BROOKES, K.,

GILL, M. and KENT, L. (2010) Evidence that genetic variation in the oxytocin receptor

(OXTR1) gene influences social cognition in ADHD. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology

and Biological Psychiatry, 34, 4, 697–702.
PARKER, H. C. (1998) The ADD Hyperactivity Handbook for Schools: Effective Strategies with

Attention Deficit Disorders in Elementary and Secondary Schools. Florida: Specialty Press.

POLANCZYK, G., DE LIMA, M. S., HORTA, B. L., BIEDERMAN, J. and ROHDE, L. A.

(2007) The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis.

The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 6, 942–948.

PASSMORE, S. (2014) The ADHD Handbook. New South Wales: Exisle Publishing.
QIU, M., YE, Z., LI, Q., LIU, G., XIE, B. and WANG, J. (2011) Changes of brain structure and

function in ADHD children. Brain Topography, 24, 3, 243–252.
QUINN, P. (2008) Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and its comorbidities in women and

girls: an evolving picture. Current Psychiatric Reports, 10, 5, 419–423.

RATEY, J. J. and HAGERMAN, E. (2008) SPARK: The Revolutionary New Science of Exercise
and the Brain. New York: Little, Brown.

REIFF, M.I. and TIPPINS, S. (2004) ADHD: a complete authoritative guide. Illinois: American
Academy of Pediatrics.

68 Support for Learning � Volume 31 � Number 1 � 2016 VC 2016 NASEN

REYNOLDS, C., VANNEST, K. and HARRISON, J. (2012) The Energetic Brain: Understanding
and Managing ADHD. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

SCIUTTO, M. and EISENBERG, M. (2007) Evaluating the evidence for and against the

overdiagnosis of ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 11, 2, 106–113.
SENIOR, J. (2009) ADHD: inclusion or exclusion in mainstream Irish schools. In S. Drudy (ed),

Education in Ireland: Challenge and Change, pp. 89–103. Dublin: Gill & Macmillan.
SHOEMAKER, K., BUNTE, T., WIEBE, S., ESPY, K., DEKOVIC, M. and MATTHYS, W.

(2012) Executive function deficits in preschool children with ADHD and DBD. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 2, 111–119.

SINGH, I. (2008) Beyond polemics: science and ethics of ADHD. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,

9, 957–964.

STEAD, J., LLOYD, G. and COHEN, D. (2006) Widening our view of ADHD. In G. Lloyd, J.

Stead and D. Cohen (eds), Critical New Perspectives on ADHD, pp. 1–11. Oxford: Routledge.

TANNOCK, R. (1998) Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: advances in cognitive,

neurobiological, and genetic research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 1,

65–99.

TAYLOR, E. (1999) Developmental neuropsychopathology of attention deficit and impulsiveness.

Development and Psychopathology, 11, 3, 607–628.

TAYLOR, E. and SONUGA-BARKE, E. (2008) Disorders of attention and activity. In M. Rutter,

D. Bishop, D. Pine, S. Scott, J. Stevenson, E. Taylor and A. Thapar (eds), Rutter’s Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry (5th ed.), pp. 521–542. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
THAPAR, A., COOPER, M., EYRE, O. and LANGLEY, K. (2013) Practitioner review: what

have we learnt about the causes of ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54,

1, 3–16.

THAPAR, A., LANGLEY, K., OWEN, M. and O’DONOVAN, M. (2007) Advances in genetic

findings on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Medicine, 37, 12,
1681–1692.

THE BRITISH PSYCHOLOGICAL SOCIETY (2000) Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

(ADHD): Guidelines and Principles for Successful Multi-agency Working. Leicester: The
British Psychological Society.

TIDEFORS, I. and STRAND, J. (2012) Life history interviews with 11 boys diagnosed with

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder who had sexually offended: a sad storyline. Journal of
Trauma and Dissociation, 13, 4, 421–434.

TIMIMI, S. and TAYLOR, E. (2004) ADHD is best understood as a cultural construct. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 184, 8–9.

VISSER, J. and JEHAN, Z. (2009) ADHD: a scientific fact or a factual opinion? A critique of

the veracity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties,
14, 2, 127–140.

WHEELER, L. (2007) Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD): Identification,
Assessment, Contextual and Curricular Variability in Boys at KS1 and KS2 in Mainstream
Schools. PhD thesis, University of Worcester in association with Coventry University.

WORLD HEALTH ORGANIZATION (1992) The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and
Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: World

Health Organization.

VC 2016 NASEN Support for Learning � Volume 31 � Number 1 � 2016 69

ZAMETKIN, A. J., NORDAHL, T. E., GROSS, M., KING, A. C., SEMPLE, W. E. and

RUMSEY J, et al (1990) Cerebral glucose metabolism in adults with hyperactivity of

childhood onset. New England Journal of Medicine, 323, 20, 1361–1366.

Correspondence
Michael Quinn

School of Education

University College Dublin (UCD)

Ireland

Email: michael.quinn@ucdconnect.ie

70 Support for Learning � Volume 31 � Number 1 � 2016 VC 2016 NASEN

Copyright of Support for Learning is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may not
be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder’s
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.

Calculate your order
Pages (275 words)
Standard price: $0.00
Client Reviews
4.9
Sitejabber
4.6
Trustpilot
4.8
Our Guarantees
100% Confidentiality
Information about customers is confidential and never disclosed to third parties.
Original Writing
We complete all papers from scratch. You can get a plagiarism report.
Timely Delivery
No missed deadlines – 97% of assignments are completed in time.
Money Back
If you're confident that a writer didn't follow your order details, ask for a refund.

Calculate the price of your order

You will get a personal manager and a discount.
We'll send you the first draft for approval by at
Total price:
$0.00
Power up Your Academic Success with the
Team of Professionals. We’ve Got Your Back.
Power up Your Study Success with Experts We’ve Got Your Back.

Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code ESSAYHELP