Education Question

DUE IN 18 HOURS

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

Video: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=vg936IW9i7Q

After watching the video you can answer one of the following questions. 

(should answer questions clearly and fully in 400-500 words)

(Make sure the answers are connected with some information from the book ch9 as I attached.)

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

1. What role does factual knowledge play in expert problem solving?

2. How can you teach problem solving and still have students develop basic skills?

3. What can teachers do to encourage creative problem solving? What types of things might impede students’ creative problem-solving?

SEVENTH

CANADIAN

EDITION

WOOLFOLK

WINNE

PERRY

EDUCATIONALPSYCHOLOG

m

what

would

you

do?

Liubomir/Shutterstoc

k

CHAPTER

9

COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

TEACHERS CASEBOOK: Uncritical Thinking

This years class is worse than any you have ever had. You assigned a research paper,

and you find more and more students are using the web for their information. In

itself, using the web is not bad, but the students appear to be completely uncritical

about what they find on the internet. If it is on the web, it must be right seems to

be the attitude of these students. Their first drafts are filled with quotes that seem

very biased to you, but there are no sources cited or listed. It is not just that students

do not know how to reference their work. You are more concerned that they cannot

critically evaluate what they are reading. And it seems all they are reading is the web!

CRITICAL THINKING

How would you help your students evaluate the information they are finding on

the web?

Beyond this immediate issue, how will you help students think more critically

about the subjects you are teaching?

How will you take into account the cultural beliefs and values of your students as

you support their critical thinking

OVERVIEW AND OBJECTIVES

In the previous chapter, we focused on how knowledge developshow people make sense

of and remember information and ideas. In this chapter, we consider complex cognitive

processes that lead to understanding. Understanding is more than memorizing. It is more

than retelling in your own words. Understanding involves appropriately transforming and

using knowledge, skills, and ideas. These understandings are considered higher-level

cognitive objectives in a commonly used system of educational objectives (L. W. Anderson

& Krathwohl,

200

1; B. S. Bloom, Engelhart, Frost, Hill, & Krathwohl, 1956). We will focus on

implications of cognitive theories for the day-to-day practice of teaching

.

Because the cognitive perspective is a philosophical orientation and not a unified

theoretical model, teaching methods derived from it are varied. In this chapter, we will first

examine the complex cognitive process of metacognitionusing knowledge and skills about

learning, motivation, and yourself to plan and regulate your own learning. Next, we explore

four important areas in which cognitive theorists have made suggestions for learning and

teaching: learning strategies, problem solving, creativity, and critical thinking, including

argumentation. Finally, we will consider the question of how to encourage the transfer of

learning from one situation to another to make learning more useful.

When you have completed this chapter, you should be able to:

9.1 Discuss the roles of metacognition in learning and remembering.

9.2 Describe several learning and study strategies that help students develop their

metacognitive abilities.

9.3 Explain the processes involved in problem solving and the factors that can interfere

with successful problem solving.

9.4 Explain how creativity is defined, assessed, and encouraged in the

classroom.

9.5 Identify factors that influence students abilities to think critically and to form and

support arguments.

9.6 Discuss how, why, and when knowledge learned in one situation might be

applied to

new

situations

and problems.

The complex cognitive skills we will examine in this chapter take us beyond the more basic

processes of perceiving, representing, and remembering (though after reading Chapter 8,

you may believe that there is nothing simple about these). Much of what we consider in

this chapter has been described as higher-order thinking, that is, thinking that moves

beyond remembering or repeating facts and ideas to truly understanding, dissecting, and

evaluating those facts or even creating new concepts and ideas of your own. Jerome Bruner

(1973) once wrote a book about this kind of thinking entitled Beyond the Information

Givena good way to describe higher-level thinking. As Bruner (1996) later noted:

Being able to go beyond the information given to figure things out is one of the few

untarnishable joys of life. One of the great triumphs of learning (and of teaching) is to get

things organized in your head in a way that permits you to know more than you ought

to. And this takes reflection, brooding about what it is that you know. (Bruner, 1996, p. 129)

In Chapter 14, you will encounter a way of thinking about higher-level thinking. We use

Blooms taxonomy to categorize levels of thinking in a hierarchy from the lower levels of

remembering, understanding, and applying, to the higher levels of analyzing, evaluating,

and creating. Of course, it is difficult to know exactly what kind of thinking any particular

student is doing without also knowing what is the basis for that thinking. A child who

invents a simple principle of balance by experimenting with a seesaw is thinking more

complexly than a student who parrots a principle of balance memorized from a textbook,

30

304 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

even though the latter might sound higher level. We are reminded

of the great scene in the 1997 Matt Damon and Ben Affleck film Good

Will Hunting: At a bar near Harvards campus, a pretentious graduate

student tries to embarrass Will Huntings uneducated friend with an

impressive analysis of history, only to be devastated when the self-taught

genius Will nails him for basing his supposed creative analysis

entirely on passages from booksgreat stuff!

METACOGNITION

In Chapter 8, we discussed a number of executive control processes,

including attention, rehearsal, organization, imagery, and elaboration.

These executive control processes are sometimes called metacogni-tive

skills, because they can be intentionally used to regulate

cognition.

METACOGNITION Metacognition sets the stage for

choosing the best way to approach a learning task.

Students with well-developed metacognitive skills set

goals, organize their activities, select among various

approaches to learning, and change strategies if needed.

Metacognitive Knowledge and Regulation

Donald Meichenbaum, professor emeritus at the University of Water-loo,

and his colleagues described metacognition as peoples aware-ness

of their own cognitive machinery and how the machinery

works (Meichenbaum, Burland, Gruson, & Cameron, 1985, p. 5).

Metacognition literally means cognition about cognitionor thinking

about thinkingsomething William James wrote about over 100

years ago (although he did not give it that name). In the Bruner

quote earlier, metacognition is involved in the reflection, brooding

about what it is that you knowthinking about your own thinking.

Metacognition is higher-order knowledge about your own thinking

as well as your ability to use this knowledge to manage your own cognitive processes

such as comprehending and problem solving (Bruning, Schraw, & Norby, 2011).

There are many metacognitive processes and skills, including judging if you have

the right knowledge to solve a problem, deciding where to focus attention, determining

if you understood what you just read, devising a plan, using strategies such as mnemon-ics,

revising the plan as you proceed, determining if you have studied enough to pass

a test, evaluating a problem solution, deciding to get help, and generally orchestrating

your cognitive powers to reach a goal (Castel et al., 2011; Meadows, 2006; Schneider,

2004). In second-language learning, you have to focus on the important elements of the

new language, ignore distracting information, and suppress what you know in the first

language that interferes or confuses learning the second language (Engel de Abreu &

Gathercole, 2012).

Metacognition involves all three kinds of knowledge we discussed earlier:

Executive control processes

Processes such as selective

attention, rehearsal, elaboration,

and organization that influence

encoding, storage, and retrieval

of information in memory.

Metacognition Knowledge about

our own thinking processes.

(1) declarative knowledge about yourself as a learner, the factors that influence your

learning and memory, and the skills, strategies, and resources needed to perform a

taskknowing what to do; (2) procedural knowledge or knowing how to use the strate-gies;

and (3) self-regulatory knowledge to ensure the completion of the taskknowing

the conditions, when and why, to apply the procedures and strategies (Bruning et al.,

2011). Metacognition is strategically applying this declarative, procedural, and self-regulatory

knowledge to accomplish goals and solve problems (Schunk, 2012). Meta-cognition

also includes knowledge about the value of applying cognitive strategies in

learning (Pressley & Harris, 2006).

Metacognition regulates thinking and learning (Brown, 1987; Nelson, 1996). There

are three essential skills: planning, monitoring, and evaluating. Planning involves decid-ing

how much time to give to a task, which strategies to use, how to start, which resources

to gather, what order to follow, what to skim and what to give intense attention to, and

so on. Monitoring is the real-time awareness of how Im doing. Monitoring is asking, Is

this making sense? Am I trying to work too fast? Have I studied enough? Evaluating

involves making judgments about the processes and outcomes of thinking and learning.

Prentice-Hall

(Pearson

Prentice-Hall)

200

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

Should I change strategies? Get help? Give up for now? Is this paper (painting, model,

poem, plan, etc.) finished? The notion of reflection in teachingthinking back on what

happened in class and why, and thinking forward to what you might do next timeis

really metacognition about teaching (Sawyer, 2006).

Of course, we do not have to be metacognitive all the time. Some actions become

routine or automatic. Metacognition is most useful when tasks are challenging, but not

too difficult. And even when we are planning, monitoring, and evaluating, these processes

are not necessarily conscious, especially in adults. We may use them automatically without

being aware of our efforts (Perner, 2000). Experts in a particular field plan, monitor, and

evaluate as second nature; they have difficulty describing their metacognitive knowledge

and skills (Pressley & Harris, 2006; Reder, 1996).

Individual Differences in Metacognition

People differ in how well and how easily they use metacognitive strategies. Some differ-ences

in metacognitive abilities are the result of development. Younger children, for

example, may not be aware of the purpose of a lessonthey may think the point is simply

to finish. They also may not be good at gauging the difficulty of a taskthey may think

reading for fun and reading a science book are the same (Gredler, 2009b). As children

grow older, they are more able to exercise executive control over strategies. For example,

they are more able to determine if they have understood instructions or if they have stud-ied

enough to remember a set of items. Metacognitive abilities begin to develop around

ages 5 to 7 and improve throughout school (Flavell, Green, & Flavell, 1995; Woolfolk &

Perry, 2015). But as we will see many times in this book, knowing and doing are not the

same. Students may know that it is better to study on a regular basis but still cram, in the

hopes of defying just once that long-established principle.

Not all differences in metacognitive abilities have to do with age or maturation (Lockl &

Schneider, 2007; Vidal-Abarca, Ma, & Gil,

2010)

. Some individual differences in meta-cognitive

abilities are probably caused by differences in biology or learning experiences.

Many students diagnosed as having learning disabilities have problems monitoring their

attention (Hallahan, Kauffman, & Pullen, 2012), particularly with long tasks. Working to

improve metacognitive skills can be especially important for students who often have

trouble in school (Schunk, 2012; Swanson, 1990).

Lessons for Teachers: Developing Metacognition

Like any knowledge or skill, metacognitive knowledge and skills can be learned and

improved.

Metacognitive Development for Younger Students. In his grade 2 classroom, Daric

Desautel (2009) worked with mostly Latin American and Asian students. As part of teaching

literacy, Desautel decided to focus on student metacognitive knowledge and skills such

as setting goals, planning, evaluating achievements, and self-reflection to help students

develop the habit of looking in at their own thinking. He also included self-reflections

to help students evaluate their writing and gain insight into themselves as readers and

writers. For example, one self-reflection included a checklist asking:

Did you pick a topic that you know all about?

Did you write a special beginning that makes the reader want more?

Did you organize your thoughts and make a Table of Contents?

Did you pick the right kind of paper and illustrate your book clearly?

Did you re-read your work to check for SOUND, SENSE, ORDER, and GOOFS?

Desautel was successful in helping all his students, not just the most verbal and advanced,

develop metacognitive knowledge. One student noted in his reflection, I worked hard

and did my best to make this book. I like nonfiction books better than stories. Next time,

I would write about a different sport (p. 2011).

In her work with grade 1 and 2 students, Nancy found that asking students two ques-tions

helped them become more metacognitive. The questions were, What did you learn

30

306 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

about yourself as a reader/writer today? and What

did you learn that you can do again and again and

again? When teachers regularly asked these ques-tions

during class, even young students demon-strated

fairly sophisticated levels of metacognitive

understanding and action (Perry, VandeKamp, &

Mercer, 2000).

Many of the cooperating teachers we work with

use a strategy called KWL to guide reading and

inquiry in general. This general frame can be used

with most grade levels. The steps are:

K What do I already know about this subject?

W What do I want to know?

L At the end of the reading or inquiry, what have I

learned?

KWL One cooperative learning strategy used by many teachers to guide

reading and inquiry is called KWL: What do I already know? What do I want

to know? What have I learned?

The KWL strategy encourages students to

look within and identify what they bring to each

learning situation, where they want to go, and

what they actually achieveda very metacognitive

approach to learning. Marilyn Friend and William

Bursuck (2002, pp. 362363) describe how one

teacher used modelling and discussion to teach the KWL strategy. After reviewing the

steps, the teacher models an example and a nonexample of using KWL to learn about

crayons.

Teacher: What do we do now that we have a passage assigned to read? First, I brainstorm,

which means I try to think of anything I already know about the topic and write it down.

The teacher writes on the board or overhead known qualities of crayons, such as made

of wax, come in many colours, can be sharpened, and several different brands.

Teacher: I then take this information I already know and put it into categories, like what

crayons are made of and crayon colours. Next, I write down any questions I would

like to have answered during my reading, such as Who invented crayons? When were

they invented? How are crayons made? Where are they made? At this point, Im ready

to read, so I read the passage on crayons. Now I must write down what I learned from

this passage. I must include any information that answers the questions I wrote down

before I read and any additional information. For example, I learned that coloured cray-ons

were first made in the United States in 1903 by Edwin Binney and E. Harold Smith.

I also learned that the Crayola Company owns the company that made the original magic

markers. Last, I must organize this information into a map so I can see the different main

points and any supporting points.

At this point, the teacher draws a map on the chalkboard or overhead.

Teacher: Lets talk about the steps I used and what I did before and after I read the passage.

A class discussion follows.

Teacher: Now Im going to read the passage again, and I want you to evaluate my text-book

reading skills based on the KWL strategy weve learned.

The teacher then proceeds to demonstrate the strategy incorrectly.

KWL A strategy to guide reading

and inquiry: BeforeWhat do I

already know? What do I want to

know? AfterWhat have I

learned?

Teacher: The passage is about crayons. Well, how much can there really be to know about

crayons besides there are hundreds of colours and they always seem to break in the

middle? Crayons are for little kids, and Im in junior high so I dont need to know that

much about them. Ill just skim the passage and go ahead and answer the question. Okay,

how well did I use the strategy steps?

The class discusses the teachers inappropriate use of the strategy. Notice how the

teacher provides both an example and a nonexamplegood teaching.

Prentice-Hall
(Pearson
Prentice-Hall)
200

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

Metacognitive Development for Secondary and Post-Secondary Students (Like You).

For older students, teachers can incorporate metacognitive questions into their lessons,

lectures, and assignments. For example, David Jonassen (2011, p. 165) suggests that

instructional designers incorporate these questions into hypermedia learning environ-ments

to help students be more self-reflective:

What are my intellectual strengths and

weaknesses?

How can I motivate myself to learn when

I need to?

How good am I at judging how well I

understand something?

How can I focus on the meaning and sig-nificance

of new information?

How can I set specific goals before I

begin a task?

Metacognition includes knowledge about using many strategies in learningour

next topic.

LEARNING STRATEGIES

Most teachers will tell you that they want their students to learn how to learn. Years of

research indicate that using good learning strategies helps students learn and that these

strategies can be taught (Hamman, Berthelot, Saia, & Crowley, 2000; Pressley & Harris,

2006). But were you taught how to learn? Powerful and sophisticated learning strategies

and study skills are seldom taught directly until high school or even college or university,

so most students have little practice with them (Winne, 2013). In contrast, early on, stu-dents

usually discover repetition and rote learning on their own, so they have extensive

practice with these strategies. And, unfortunately, some teachers think that memorizing

is learning (Beghetto, 2008; Woolfolk Hoy & Murphy, 2001). This may explain why many

students cling to flash cards and memorizingthey do not know what

else

to do

(Willoughby, Porter, Belsito, & Yearsley, 1999).

As you saw in Chapter 8, the way something is learned in the first place greatly influ-ences

how readily we remember the information and how appropriately we can apply

the knowledge later. First, students must be cognitively engaged in order to learn; they

have to focus attention on the relevant or important aspects of the material. Second, they

have to invest effort, make connections, elaborate, translate, invent, organize, and reorgan-ize

to think and process deeplythe greater the practice and processing, the stronger the

learning. Finally, students must regulate and monitor their own learningkeep track of

what is making sense and notice when a new approach is needed; they must be metacog-nitive.

The emphasis today is on helping students develop effective learning strategies

that focus attention and effort, process information deeply, and monitor understanding.

Being Strategic about Learning

Learning strategies are flexible kinds of procedural knowledgeknowing how to do some-thing.

There are thousands of strategies. Some are general and taught in school, such as

summarizing or outlining. Others are specific to a subject, such as using a mnemonic to

remember the order of the planets: My Very Educated Mother Just Served Us Nachos for

Mercury, Venus, Earth, Mars, Jupiter, Saturn, Uranus, and Neptune. Other strategies may

be unique, invented by an individual to learn Chinese characters, for example. Learning

strategies can be cognitive (summarizing, identifying the main idea), metacognitive (moni-toring

comprehensionDo I understand?), or behavioural (using an internet dictionary,

setting a timer to work until times up) (Cantrell, Almasi, Carter, Rintamaa, & Madden,

2010). All are ways of accomplishing a learning task that are intentionally applied when

usual methods have not worked and strategic effort is needed (K. R. Harris, Alexander, &

Graham, 2008). Over time, as you become more expert at using the strategies, you need

What questions should I ask about the material

before I begin?

How well have I accomplished my goals

once Im finished?

Have I learned as much as I could have once

I finish a task?

Have I considered all options after I solve a

problem?

307

Learning strategies A special

kind

of procedural knowledgeknowing

how to approach

learning tasks

308 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

less intentional effort. Ultimately you may become more automatic in applying the strate-gies;

in other words, the strategies will become your usual way of accomplishing that kind

of task, until they do not work and you need new strategies.

Skilled learners have a wide range of learning strategies that they can apply fairly

automatically. Using learning strategies and study skills is related to higher grade-point

averages (GPAs) in high school and persistence in college or university (Robbins et al.,

2004; Winne, 2013). Researchers have identified several important principles:

1. Students should be exposed to a number of different strategies, not only general

learning strategies but also very specific strategies for particular subjects, such as the

graphic strategies described later in this section.

2. Students should be taught self-regulatory (conditional) knowledge about when,

where, and why to use various strategies. Although this may seem obvious, teachers

often neglect this step. A strategy is more likely to be maintained and employed if

students know when, where, and why to use it.

3. Students may know when and how to use a strategy, but unless they also develop the

desire to employ these skills, general learning ability will not improve. Remember, left

to their own, many students, adult students included, do not choose the most effec-tive

strategies, even if they know how to do the strategy (Son & Simon, 2012). Several

learning strategy programs include a motivational training component.

4. Students need to believe that they can learn new strategies, that the effort will pay

off, and that they can get smarter by applying these strategies.

5. Students need some background knowledge and useful schemas in the area being

studied to make sense of learning materials. It will be difficult to find the main idea

in a paragraph about ichthyology, for example, if you do not know much about fish.

So students may need direct instruction in schematic (content) knowledge along with

strategy training. Table 9.1 summarizes several learning strategies.

Deciding What Is Important. You can see from the first entry in Table 9.1 that learn-ing

begins with focusing attentiondeciding what is important. But distinguishing the

main idea from less important information is not always easy. Often students focus on

the seductive details or the concrete examples, perhaps because these are more inter-esting

(Gardner, Brown, Sanders, & Menke, 1992). You may have had the experience

TABLE 9.1 Examples of Learning Strategies

EXAMPLES

Planning and Focusing Attention Setting goals and timetables

Underlining and highlighting

Skimming, looking for headings and topic sentences

Organizing and Remembering

Comprehension

Making organizational charts

Creating flowcharts, Venn diagrams

Using mnemonics, imagery

Concept mapping, webs

Cognitive Monitoring

Summarizing, outlining, and note taking

Creating examples

Explaining to a peer

Making predictions

Self-questioning and self-testing

Identifying what does not make sense

Practice Using part practice

Using whole practic

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

of remembering a joke or an intriguing example from a lecture, but not being clear about

the larger point the professor was presenting. Finding the central idea is especially dif-ficult

if you lack prior knowledge in an area and if the amount of new information pro-vided

is extensive. Teachers can give students practice noticing and using signals in texts

such as headings, bold words, outlines, or other indicators to identify key concepts and

main ideas (Lorch, Lorch, Ritchey, McGovern, & Coleman, 2001).

Summaries. Creating summaries can help students learn, but students have to be taught

how to summarize (Byrnes, 1996; Palincsar & Brown, 1984). Jeanne Ormrod (2012) sum-marizes

these suggestions for helping students create summaries. Ask students to:

Find or create a topic sentence for each paragraph or section

Identify big ideas that cover several specific points

Find some supporting information for each big idea

Delete any redundant information or unnecessary details

Begin by doing summaries of short, easy, well-organized readings. Introduce longer,

less organized, and more difficult passages gradually. Initially it may be useful to provide

a scaffold such as: This paragraph is about __________ and __________. They are the same

in these ways: __________, but different in these ways: __________. Ask students to com-pare

their summaries and discuss what ideas they thought were important and whywhat

is their evidence?

Two other study strategies that are based on identifying key ideas are underlining

texts and taking notes.

STOP & THINK How do you make notes as you read? Look back over the past several pages

of this chapter. Are my words highlighted yellow or pink? Are there marks or drawings in the

margins, and if so, do the notes pertain to the chapter content or are they grocery lists and

doodles?

Underlining and Highlighting. Do you underline or highlight key phrases in textbooks?

Underlining and note taking are probably two of the most frequent but ineffectively used

strategies among post-secondary students. One common problem is that students under-line

or highlight too much. It is far better to be selective. Research in Phils lab (Winne

et al., 2017) indicates you have about twice as much chance to remember what you high-light

compared to what you do not. Of course, that is a good thing only if you highlight

information you need to know. In studies that limit how much students can underlinefor

example, only one sentence per paragraphlearning has improved (Snowman, 1984). In

addition to being selective, you also should actively transform the information into your

own words as you underline or take notes. Do not rely on the words of the book. Note

connections between what you are reading and other things you already know. Draw

diagrams to illustrate relationships. Finally, look for organizational patterns in the material,

and use them to guide your underlining or note taking.

Taking Notes. Taking good lecture notes is not an easy task. You have to hold the lec-ture

information in working memory; select, organize, and transform the important ideas

and themes before the information falls off your working memory workbench; and write

down the ideas and themesall while you are still following the lecture (Bui, Myerson,

& Hale, 2013; Kobayashi, 2005; Peverly et al., 2007). As you fill your notebook with words

and try to keep up with a lecturer, you may wonder if taking notes makes a difference. It

does, if the strategy is used well.

Taking notes focuses attention during class. Of course, if taking notes distracts you

from actually listening to and making sense of the lecture, then note taking may not

be effective (Kiewra, 1989, 2002; Van Meter, Yokoi, & Pressley, 1994). Dung Bui and

his colleagues (2013) found that taking organized notes worked well for students with

good working memory, but using a laptop to transcribe lectures worked better for

students with poor working memories, at least for short lectures.

30

31

0 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

Taking organized notes makes you construct meaning from what you are hearing, seeing,

or reading, so you elaborate, translate into your own words, and remember (Armbruster,

2000). Even if students do not review notes before a test, taking them in the first place

appears to aid learning, especially for those who lack prior knowledge in an area.

Notes provide extended external storage that allows you to return and review. Students

who use their notes to study tend to perform better on tests, especially if they take

many high-quality notesmore is better as long as you are capturing key ideas, con-cepts,

and relationships, not just intriguing details (Kiewra, 1985, 1989; Peverly, Brobst,

Graham, & Shaw, 2003).

Expert students match notes to their anticipated use and modify strategies after tests

or assignments, use personal codes to flag material that is unfamiliar or difficult, fill in

holes by consulting relevant sources (including other students in the class), and record

information verbatim only when a verbatim response will be required. In other words,

they are strategic about taking and using notes (Van Meter et al., 1994).

Even with these advantages, remember the caveat mentioned earlier. It is possible

that taking well-organized notes that capture the important ideas in lecture is easier for

students with better working memory abilities. When students have more limited working

memories, they might need to focus on understanding the teacher and transcribing as

much as possible, as long as they are fast typists.

Even though taking notes is valuable from middle school through graduate school,

students with learning disabilities often have difficulty with the strategy (Boyle, 2010a,

2010b). Middle school and high school students with learning disabilities who used a

strategic note-taking form recalled and understood significantly more key ideas from sci-ence

lectures than students in control groups who used conventional note-taking methods

(Boyle, 2010b; Boyle & Weishaar, 2001). For an example of this kind of form, see www.

ldonline.org/article/6210/.

Figure 9.1 is a general form that can be used in many note-taking situations. Dividing

up the page is an idea from the Cornell Notes system, devised by Walter Pauk of Cornell

University, who wrote the classic guide, How to Study in College in the 1950s. It is still

available (Pauk & Owens, 2010). This form could be useful for any student who needs

extra guidance in note taking.

Visual Tools for Organizing

To use underlining and note taking effectively, you must identify main ideas. In addition, you

must understand the organization of the text or lecturethe connections and relationships

among ideas. Some visual strategies have been developed to help students with this key

organizational element (Van Meter, 2001). Concept maps are graphical tools for organizing

and representing knowledge and relationships within a particular field or on a given topic

(Hagemans, van der Meij, & de Jong, 2013; van der Meij, 2012). Figure 9.2 on page 312 is a

concept map of a website for creating concept maps by the Institute for Human and Machine

Cognition Cmap tools. You may have referred to these interconnected ideas as webs.

In a review of 55 studies with students from grade 4 to graduate school, and subjects

ranging from science to statistics to nursing, John Nesbit and Olusola Adesopes (2006)

research at Simon Fraser University concluded that, In comparison with activities such as

reading text passages, attending lectures, and participating in class discussions, concept map-ping

activities are more effective for attaining knowledge retention and transfer (p. 4

34

).

Having students map relationships by noting causal connections, comparison/contrast con-nections,

and examples improves recall. Anitas students used Cmaps, the free downloadable

tools from the website shown in Figure 9.2 for creating concept mapsone student even

planned his dissertation and organized all the reading for his doctoral examinations with

tools from the website. Computer Cmaps can be linked to the internet, and students in dif-ferent

classrooms and schools all over the world can collaborate on them. Students should

compare their filled-in maps and discuss the differences in their thinking with each other.

Instructor-provided maps can serve as guides for studying. Mieke Hagemans and her

colleagues (2013) found that colour-coded concept maps helped high school physics

students master complex concepts. The concept maps were part of a computer program

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

FIGURE 9.1

A FORM FOR TAKING NOTES MORE STRATEGICALLY

Topic: What do I already know about this topic?

311

Key Points / Key

Terms

Notes

Summaries: Write 3 to 5 sentences that capture the main ideas.

1.

2.

3.

4.

5.

Questions: What is still confusing or unclear?

Source: Based on ideas from Pauk, Walter; Owens, Ross J. Q. (2010), How to Study in College (10th ed.). (Original work published 1962)

Florence, KY: Cengage Learning; and http://lsc.cornell.edu/LSC_ Resources/cornellsystem , Pearson Education.

The maps changed colour as the students completed study in that section of the map, so

students had a scaffold to guide them through the reading and assignments and even

remind them, for example, that they had not spent enough time on the assignments on

acceleration in their study of velocity.

There are other ways to visualize organization, such as Venn diagrams, which show

how ideas or concepts overlap, and tree diagrams, which show how ideas branch off each

other. Timelines organize information in sequence and are useful in classes such as history

or geology

312 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

FIGURE 9.2

THE WEBSITE FOR THE INSTITUTE FOR HUMAN AND MACHINE

COGNITION CMAP TOOLS

At the Institute for Human and Machine Cognition website, you can download concept mapping tools to construct, share, and criticize

knowledge on any subject: cmap.imhc.us.

Source: Institute for Human and Machine Cognition Cmap Tools. Retrieved from http://cmap.ihmc.us. Reprinted with permission from the IHMC

Reading Strategies

As we saw earlier, effective learning strategies should help students focus attention,

invest effort (connect, elaborate, translate, organize, summarize) so they process infor-mation

deeply, and monitor their understanding. A number of strategies support these

processes in reading. Many strategies use mnemonics to help students remember the

steps involved. For example, one strategy that can be used for any grade above later

elementary is READS:

R Review headings and subheadings.

E Examine boldface words.

A Ask, What do I expect to learn?

D Do itRead!

S Summarize in your own words. (Friend & Bursuck, 2012)

A strategy that can be used in reading literature is CAPS:

C Who are the characters?

A What is the aim of the story?

P What problem happens?

S How is the problem solved

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

These strategies are effective for several reasons. First, following the steps makes

students more aware of the organization of a given chapter. How often have you skipped

reading headings entirely and thus missed major clues about the way the information was

organized? Next, these steps require students to study the chapter in sections instead of

trying to learn all the information at once. This makes use of distributed practice. Answer-ing

questions about the material forces students to process the information more deeply

and with greater elaboration.

No matter what strategies you use, students have to be taught how to use them.

Direct teaching, explanation, modelling, and practice with feedback are necessary and are

especially important for students with learning challenges and students whose first lan-guage

is not English. For an example of direct teaching of strategies with explanations,

modelling, and practice with feedback, see the KWL discussion on page 306 of this

chapter.

Applying Learning Strategies

One of the most common findings in research on learning strategies is a phenomenon

known as production deficiencies. Students learn strategies, but do not apply them when

they could or should (Pressley & Harris, 2006; Son & Simon, 2012). This is especially a

problem for students with learning disabilities. For these students, executive control pro-cesses

(metacognitive strategies) such as planning, organizing, monitoring progress, and

making adaptations, often are underdeveloped (Kirk, Gallagher, Anastasiow, & Coleman,

2006). It makes sense to teach these strategies directly. To ensure that students actually

use the strategies they learn, several conditions must be met.

Appropriate Tasks. First, of course, the learning task must be appropriate. Why

would students use more complex learning strategies when the task set by the teacher

is to learn and return the exact words of the text or lecture? With these tasks, teach-ers

reward memorizing, and the best strategies involve distributed practice and perhaps

mnemonics (described in Chapter 8). But hopefully, contemporary teachers use few of

these kinds of tasks, so if the task is understanding, not memorizing, what else is

necessary?

Valuing Learning. The second condition for using sophisticated strategies is that

students must care about learning and understanding. They must have goals that can

be reached using effective strategies (Zimmerman & Schunk, 2001). Anita was reminded

of this one semester in her educational psychology class when she enthusiastically

shared an article about study skills from a national newspaper. The gist of the article

was that students should continually revise and rewrite their notes from a course, so

that by the end, all their understanding could be captured in one or two pages. Of

course, the majority of the knowledge at that point would be reorganized and con-nected

well with other knowledge. See, she told the class, these ideas are realnot

just trapped in texts. They can help you study smarter. After a heated discussion, one

of the best students said in exasperation, Im carrying 18 hoursI dont have time to

learn this stuff! She did not believe that her goalto survive the 18-hour course

loadcould be reached by using time-consuming study strategies, and she might have

been right.

Effort and Efficacy. Anitas busy student also was concerned about effort. The third

condition for applying learning strategies is that students must believe the effort and

investment required to apply the strategies are reasonable, given the likely return (Winne,

2001). And of course, students must believe they are capable of using the strategies; they

must have self-efficacy for using the strategies to learn the material in question (Schunk,

2012). This is related to another condition: Students must have a base of knowledge and/

or experience in the area. No learning strategies will help students accomplish tasks that

are completely beyond their current understandings.

The Guidelines: Becoming an Expert Student provide a summary of ideas for you

and your students.

313

Production deficiencies Failing

to activate a learning strategya

productionwhen it is appropriate

and useful to use the strategy

314 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

Becoming an Expert Student

GUIDELINES

Be clear about your goals in studying.

Examples

1. Survey readings to target specific concepts on which you

will focus.

2. Write the introduction section of a paper.

Make sure you have the necessary declarative knowledge

(facts, concepts, ideas) to understand new information.

Examples

1. Keep definitions of key vocabulary available as you study.

2. Use your general knowledge. Ask yourself, What do I

already know about ________?

3. Build your vocabulary by learning two or three new words a

day using them in everyday conversation.

Find out what type of test the teacher will give (essay, short

answer), and study the material with that in mind.

Examples

1. For a test with detailed questions, practice writing answers to

possible questions.

2. For a multiple-choice test, use mnemonics to remember

definitions of key terms.

Make sure you are familiar with the organization of the

materials to be learned.

Examples

1. Preview the headings, introductions, topic sentences, and

summaries of the text.

2. Be alert for words and phrases that signal relationships,

such as on the other hand, because, first, second,

however, since.

Know your own cognitive skills, and use them deliberately.

Examples

1. Use examples and analogies to relate new material to

something you care about and understand well, such as

sports, hobbies, or films.

2. If one study technique is not working, try anotherthe goal

is to stay involved, not to use any particular strategy.

3. If you start to daydream, stand up from your desk and face

away from your books, but do not leave. Then sit back down

and study.

Study the right information in a productive

way.

Examples

1. Be sure you know exactly what topics and readings the test

will cover.

2. Spend your time on the important, difficult, and unfamiliar

material that will be required for the test or assignment.

Resist the temptation to go over what you already know well,

even if that feels good.

3. Keep a list of the parts of the text that give you trouble, and

spend more time on those pages.

4. Process the important information thoroughly by using

mnemonics, forming images, creating examples, answering

questions, making notes in your own words, and elaborating

on the text. Do not try to memorize the authors wordsuse

your own.

Monitor your own comprehension.

Examples

1. Use questioning to check your understanding.

2. When reading speed slows down, decide if the

information

in the passage is important. If it is, note the problem so you

can re-read or get help to understand. If it is not important,

ignore it.

3. Check your understanding by working with a friend and

quizzing one

another.

Manage your time.

Examples

1. When is your best time for studying? Morning, late at night?

Study your most difficult subjects then.

2. Study in shorter rather than longer blocks, unless you are

really engaged and making great progress.

3. Eliminate time wasters and distractions. Study in a room

without a television or your roommate, then turn off your

phone and maybe even your connection to the internet.

4. Use bonus timetake your educational psychology notes to

the doctors office waiting room or laundromat. You will use

time well and avoid reading old magazines.

Based on ideas from: https://ucc.vt.edu/academic_support/study_skills_

information.html; Wong, L. (2015). Essential study skills (8th ed.) Stamford,

CT: Cengage., Pearson Education.

Reaching Every Student: Learning Strategies for Struggling Students

Reading is key in all learning. Strategy instruction can help many struggling readers. As

you have seen, some approaches make use of mnemonics to help students remember the

steps. For example, Susan Cantrell and her colleagues identified 862 students in grades 6

and 9 who were at least two years behind in reading (Cantrell, Almasi, Carter, Rintamaa,

& Madden, 2010). The students were from 23 different schools. Students were randoml

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

TABLE 9.2 Teaching Strategies for Improving Students Metacognitive Knowledge

and Skills

These eight guidelines taken from Pressley and Woloshyn (1995) should help you in teaching any

metacognitive strategy.

Teach a few strategies at a time, intensively and extensively, as part of the ongoing curriculum.

Model and explain new strategies.

If parts of the strategy were not understood, model again and re-explain strategies in ways

that are sensitive to those confusing or misunderstood aspects of strategy use.

Explain to students where and when to use the strategy.

Provide plenty of practice, using strategies for as many appropriate tasks as possible.

Encourage students to monitor how they are doing when they are using strategies.

Increase students motivation to use strategies by heightening their awareness that they are

acquiring valuable skillsskills that are at the heart of competent functioning.

Emphasize reflective processing rather than speedy processing; do everything possible to

eliminate high anxiety in students; encourage students to shield themselves from distractions

so they can attend to academic tasks.

For a list of strategies and how to teach them see unl.edu/csi/bank.html.

Source: Based on Pressley, M., & Woloshyn, V. (1995). Cognitive Strategy Instruction That Really Improves Childrens

Academic Performance. Cambridge, MA: Brookline Books., Pearson Education.

315

assigned to either a Learning Strategies Curriculum (Deshler & Schumaker, 2005) or the

traditional curriculum. The Learning Strategies Curriculum focused on six strategies: word

identification, visual imagery, self-questioning, LINCS vocabulary strategy, sentence writ-ing,

and paraphrasing. The LINCS vocabulary strategy uses stories and imagery to help

students learn how to identify, organize, define, and remember words, which increases

their ownership of their learning. The LINCS steps are:

L List the partsidentify the vocabulary word and key information.

I Identify a reminding wordpick a known word that reminds them of the vocabulary

word.

N Note a LINCing storycreate a story that bridges the vocabulary word with the known

word.

C Create a LINCing picturedraw a picture that represents the story.

S Self-testcheck their learning of the vocabulary word by reciting all the parts of their

LINCS.

After a year, the grade 6 students who had participated in the Learning Strategies

Curriculum performed significantly better on reading comprehension and strategy use,

but there were no differences for grade 9 students. It is possible that reading strategy

instruction is most effective in elementary and early middle school, when students are

learning how to learn through reading (Cantrell et al., 2010).

Of course, you have to do more than just tell students about the strategyyou have

to teach it. Michael Pressley, formerly of the University of Western Ontario, and his col-league

Vera Woloshyn at Brock University (1995) developed the Cognitive Strategies Model

as a guide for teaching students to improve their metacognitive strategies. Table 9.2

describes the steps in teaching these strategies.

PROBLEM SOLVING

STOP & THINK You are interviewing with the district superintendent for a position as a school

psychologist. The superintendent is known for his unorthodox interview questions. He hands

you a pad of paper and a ruler and says, Tell me, what is the exact thickness of a single sheet

of paper?

316 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

The Stop & Think is a true story. Anita was asked the paper thickness question in an

interview years ago. The answer was to measure the thickness of the entire pad and divide

by the number of pages in the pad. She got the answer and the job, but what a tense

moment that was. The superintendent was probably interested in her ability to solve

problemsunder pressure!

A problem has an initial state (the current situation), a goal (the desired outcome),

and a path for reaching the goal (the operations or activities that move you toward the

goal). Problem solvers often have to set and reach subgoals as they move toward the final

solution. For example, if your goal is to drive to the beach, but at the first stop sign you

skid through the intersection, you may have to reach a subgoal of fixing your brakes before

you can continue toward the original goal (Schunk, 2012). Also, problems can range from

well-structured to ill-structured, depending on how clear-cut the goals are and how much

structure is provided for solving them. Most arithmetic problems are well structured, but

finding the right university major or career is ill-structuredmany different solutions and

paths to solutions are possible. Life presents many ill-structured problems (Belland, 2011).

Problem solving is usually defined as formulating new answers, going beyond the

simple application of previously learned rules to achieve a goal. Problem solving is what

happens when no solution is obviouswhen, for example, you cannot afford new brakes

for the car that skidded on the way to the beach (Mayer & Wittrock, 2006). Some psycholo-gists

suggest that most human learning involves problem solving and that helping students

become better problem solvers is one of educations greatest challenges (Anderson, 2010;

Greiff et al., 2013). Solving complex, ill-structured problems is one key ability measured

by the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA), a comprehensive world-wide

assessment of reading, mathematics, and science for 15-year-olds. In the results for

the 2015 exams, Canada ranked 10 out of 74 countries in math, 3 in problem-solving

performance, and 7 in science.

There is a debate about problem solving. Some psychologists believe that effective

problem-solving strategies are specific to the problem area or domain. For example, the

problem-solving strategies in mathematics are unique to math, the strategies in art are

unique to art, and so on. The other side of the debate claims that there are some general

problem-solving strategies that can be useful in many areas. General problem-solving

strategies usually include the steps of identifying the problem, setting goals, exploring

possible solutions and consequences, acting, and finally evaluating the outcome.

There is evidence for the value of both general and specific strategies. In their

research with grades 4 and 5 students, Steven Hecht and Kevin Vagi (2010) found that

both domain-specific and general factors affected performance on problems involving

fractions. The influences were specific conceptual knowledge about fractions and the

general information-processing skill of attentive classroom behaviour. Other studies with

elementary school students found that both specific arithmetic knowledge and general

attention-focusing, working memory, and oral language skills were related to arithmetic

problem solving (Fuchs et al., 2006, 2012, 2013).

People appear to move between general and specific approaches, depending on the

situation and their level of expertise. Early on, when we know little about a problem area

or domain, we can rely on general learning and problem-solving strategies to make sense

of the situation. As we gain more domain-specific knowledge (particularly procedural

knowledge about how to do things in the domain), we consciously apply the general

strategies less and less; our problem solving becomes more automatic. But if we encounter

a problem outside our current knowledge, we may return to relying on general strategies

to attack the problem (Alexander, 1992, 1996).

A key first step in any problem solvinggeneral or specificis identifying that a

problem exists (and perhaps treating the problem as an opportunity).

Problem solving Creating new

solutions for problems.

Identifying: Problem Finding

Problem identification is not always straightforward. We are reminded of a story about

tenants who were angry because the elevators in their building were slow. Consultants

hired to fix the problem reported that the elevators were no worse than average an

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

improvements would be very expensive. One day, as the building supervisor watched

people waiting impatiently for an elevator, he realized that the problem was not slow

elevators, but the fact that people were bored; they had nothing to do while they waited.

When the boredom problem was identified and seen as an opportunity to improve the

waiting experience, the simple solution of installing a mirror by the elevator on each

floor eliminated complaints.

Even though problem identification is a critical first step, research indicates that people

often leap to naming the first problem that comes to mind (the elevators are too slow!).

Experts in a field are more likely to spend time carefully considering the nature of the

problem (Bruning et al., 2011). Finding a solvable problem and turning it into an opportu-nity

is the process behind many successful inventions, such as the ballpoint pen, garbage

disposal, appliance timer, alarm clock, self-cleaning oven, and thousands of others.

Once a solvable problem is identified, what next?

Defining Goals and Representing

the Problem

Lets take a real problem: The machines designed to pick tomatoes are damaging the

tomatoes. What should we do? If we represent the problem as a faulty machine design,

then the goal is to improve the machine. But if we represent the problem as a faulty design

of the tomatoes, then the goal is to develop a tougher tomato. The problem-solving pro-cess

follows two entirely different paths, depending on which representation and goal are

chosen (Nokes-Malach & Mestre, 2013). To represent the problem and set a goal, you have

to focus attention on relevant information, understand the words of the problem, and

activate the right schema to understand the whole problem.

STOP & THINK If you have black socks and white socks in your drawer, mixed in the ratio of

four-to-five, how many socks will you have to take out to make sure you have a pair the same

colour? (Adapted from Sternberg & Davidson, 1982)

Focusing Attention on What Is Relevant. Representing the problem often requires find-ing

the relevant information and ignoring the irrelevant details. For example, what infor-mation

was relevant in solving the sock problem in Stop & Think? Did you realize that the

information about the four-to-five ratio of black socks to white socks is irrelevant? As long

as you have only two different colours of socks in the drawer, you will have to remove

only three socks before two of them match.

Understanding the Words. The second task in representing a problem is understand-ing

the meaning of the words, sentences, and factual information in the problem. So

problem solving requires comprehension of the language and relations in the problem.

In math word problems, it also involves assigning mathematical operators (addition,

division, etc.) to relations among numbers (Jitendra et al., 2009; Lee, Ng, & Ng, 2009).

All this makes a demand on working memory. For example, the main stumbling block

in representing many word problems and problems with fractions is the students under-standing

of partwhole relations (Fuchs et al., 2013). Students have trouble figuring out

what is part of what, as is evident in this dialogue between a teacher and a student in

grade 1:

Teacher: Pete has three apples. Ann also has some apples. Pete and Ann have nine apples

altogether. How many apples does Ann have?

Student: Nine.

Teacher: Why?

Student: Because you just said so.

Teacher: Can you retell the story?

Student: Pete had three apples. Ann also had some apples. Ann had nine apples. Pete also

has nine apples. (Adapted from De Corte & Verschaffel, 1985, p. 19, Pearson Education)

The student interprets altogether (the whole) as each (the parts).

31

318 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

A common difficulty for older students is understanding that ratio and proportion

problems are based on multiplicative relations, not additive relations (Jitendra et al., 2009).

So to solve

2 : 14 = ? : 35

many students subtract to find the difference between 2 and 14 (14 2 = 12) and then

subtract 12 from 35 to get 23, giving them the (wrong) answer

2 : 14 = 23 : 35

The real question is about the proportional relationship between 2 and 14. How many

times larger than 2 is 14? The answer: 7 times larger. Then the real question is 35 is

7 times larger than what number? The answer is 5 (7 5 = 35). So

2 : 14 = 5 : 35

Understanding the Whole Problem. The third task in representing a problem is to

assemble all the relevant information and sentences into an accurate understanding or

translation of the total problem. This means that students need to form a conceptual model

of the problemthey have to understand what the problem is really asking (Jonassen,

2003). Consider the example of the trains in the Stop & Think.

STOP & THINK Two train stations are 50 km apart. At 2 p.m. one Saturday afternoon, two

trains start toward each other, one from each station. Just as the trains pull out of the stations,

a bird springs into the air in front of the first train and flies ahead to the front of the second train.

When the bird reaches the second train, it turns back and flies toward the first train. The bird

continues to do this until the trains meet. If both trains travel at the rate of 25 kph and the bird

flies at 100 kph, how many kilometres will the bird have flown before the trains meet? (Posner,

1973)

Your interpretation of the problem is called a translation because you translate the

problem into a schema that you understand. If you translate this as a distance problem

(activate a distance schema) and set a goal (I have to figure out how far the bird travels

before it meets the oncoming train and turns around, then how far it travels before it has

to turn again, and finally add up all the trips back and forth), then you have a very dif-ficult

task on your hands. But there is a better way to structure the problem. You can

represent it as a question of time and focus on the time the bird is in the air. The solution

could be stated like this:

The trains are going the same speed so they will meet in the middle, 25 km from each

station. This will take one hour because they are travelling 25 kph. In an hour, the bird

will cover 100 km because it is flying at 100 km per hour. Easy!

Research shows that students can be too quick to decide what a problem is asking.

Once a problem is categorizedAha, its a distance problem!a particular schema is

activated. The schema directs attention to relevant information and sets up expectations

for what the right answer should look like. For example, if you use a distance schema in

the above problem, the right answer looks like adding up many small distance calculations

(Kalyuga, Chandler, Tuovinen, & Sweller, 2001; Reimann & Chi, 1989).

When students lack the necessary schemas to represent problems, they often rely

on surface features of the situation and represent the problem incorrectly, like the student

who wrote 15 + 24 = 39 as the answer to, Joan has 15 bonus points and Louise has

24. How many more does Louise have? This student saw two numbers and the word

more, so he applied the add to get more procedure. Focus on surface features often hap-pens

when students are taught to search for keywords (more, less, greater, etc.), pick a

strategy or formula based on the keywords (more means add), and apply the formula.

Actually, focusing on surface features gets in the way of forming a conceptual under-standing

of the whole problem and using the right schema (Van de Walle, Karp, & Bay-Williams,

2010)

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

FIGURE 9.3

FOUR DIFFERENT WAYS TO REPRESENT A PROBLEM

A teacher asks, How many wildlife stamps will Jane need to fill her book if there are three pages and

each page holds 30 stamps? The teacher gives the students supplies such as a squared paper, number

lines, and place-value frames and encourages them to think of as many ways as possible to solve the

problem. Here are four different solutions, based on four different but correct representations.

JIM:

30
30

+30

90

MARIAH:

tens

JOE:

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

90 stamps

ones

90 stamps

PHYLLIS:

30

319

30
90 stamps

Source: Riedesel, C. A, & Schwartz, J. E. (1999). Essentials of Elementary Mathematics, (2nd ed). Upper Reprinted by permission of

Pearson Education, Inc., Upper Saddle River, NJ., Pearson Education.

30

When students use the wrong schema, they overlook critical information, use irrel-evant

information, and may even misread or misremember critical information so that it

fits the schema. But when students use the proper schema to represent a problem, they

are less likely to be confused by irrelevant information or tricky wording, such as the

presence of the word more in a problem that really requires subtraction (Fenton, 2007;

Resnick, 1981). Figure 9.3 gives examples of different ways students might represent a

simple mathematics problem. Exposure to different ways of representing and solving

problems helps develop mathematical understanding (Star & Rittle-Johnson, 2009).

How can students who lack a good base of knowledge improve their translation and

schema selection? To answer this question, we usually have to move to area-specific

problem-solving strategies because schemas are specific to content areas.

Translation and Schema Training: Direct Instruction in Schemas. For students with

little knowledge in an area, teachers can begin by directly teaching the necessary schema

using demonstration, modelling, and think-alouds. As we just saw, ratio/proportion

problems like the following are a big challenge for many students.

Ernesto and Dawn worked separately on their social studies projects this weekend. The

ratio of the number of hours Ernesto spent on the project to the number of hours Dawn

spent on the project was 2:3. If Ernesto spent 16 hours on the project, how many hours

did Dawn spend on the project? (Jitendra et al., 2009, p. 257)

The teacher used a think-aloud to focus students on the key schema for solving

this problem, so she said, First, I figure this is a ratio problem, because it compared the

number of hours that Ernesto worked to the number of hours Dawn worked. This is a

part-part ratio that tells about a multiplicative relationship (2:3) between the hours

Ernesto and Dawn worked. The teacher went on to think aloud, Next, I represented the

information. . . . Finally, I used the equivalent fractions strategy and. . . . The think-aloud

demonstration can be followed by providing students with many worked examples. In

mathematics and physics it appears that in the early stages of learning, students benefit

from seeing many different kinds of example problems worked out correctly for them

(Moreno, Ozogul, & Reisslein, 2011). But before we explore worked examples in the next

section, a caution is in order. Students with advanced knowledge improve when they solve

new problems, not when they focus on already-worked examples. Worked examples ca

32

0 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

actually interfere with the learning of more expert

students. This has been called the expert reversal

effect because what works for experts is the reverse

of what works for beginners (Kalyuga & Renkl, 2010;

Kalyuga, Rikers, & Paas, 2012).

WORKED EXAMPLES Students benefit from seeing many different kinds of

example problems worked out correctly for them, especially when they show

an expert problem solvers thinking at critical steps.

Translation and Schema Training: Worked Examples.

Worked examples reflect all the stages of problem

solvingidentifying the problem, setting goals,

exploring solutions, solving the problem, and finally

evaluating the outcome (Schworm & Renkl, 2007;

van Gog, Paas, & Sweller, 2010). Worked examples

are useful in many subject areas. Adrienne Lee and

Laura Hutchinson (1998) found that undergraduate

students learned more when they were provided

with examples of chemistry problem solutions that

were annotated to show an expert problem solvers

thinking at critical steps. In Australia, Slava Kalyuga

and colleagues (2001) found that worked examples

helped apprentices to learn about electrical circuits

when the apprentices had less experience in the

area. Silke Schworm and Alexander Renkl (2007)

used video examples to help student teachers learn how to make convincing arguments

for or against a position.

Why are examples effective? Part of the answer is in cognitive load theory, discussed

in the previous chapter. When students lack specific knowledge in domainsfor example,

fractions or proportionsthey try to solve the problems using general strategies such as

looking for key words or applying rote procedures. But these approaches put great strain

on working memorytoo much to keep in mind at once overloads memory. In contrast,

worked examples chunk some of the steps, provide cues and feedback, focus attention

on relevant information, and make fewer demands on memory, so the students can use

cognitive resources to understand instead of searching randomly for solutions (Wittwer &

Renkl, 2010). It is especially useful if the examples focus on critical features of the prob-lems

that students have not yet mastered (Guo, Pang, Yang, & Ding, 2012).

To get the most benefit from worked examples, however, students have to actively

engagejust looking over the examples is not enough. This is not too surprising when

you think about what supports learning and memory. You need to pay attention, process

deeply, and connect with what you already know. Students should explain the examples

to themselves. This self-explanation component is a critical part of making learning from

worked examples active, not passive. Examples of self-explanation strategies include try-ing

to predict the next step in a solution, then checking to see if you are right or trying

to identify an underlying principle that explains how to solve the problem. In their study

with student teachers, Schworm and Renkl (2007) embedded prompts that required the

student teachers to think about and explain elements of the arguments they saw on the

tape, such as, Which argumentative elements does this sequence contain? How is it

related to Kirstens statement? (p. 289). Students have to be mentally engaged in making

sense of the examples, and self-explanation is one key to engagement (Atkinson & Renkl,

2007; Wittwer & Renkl, 2010).

Another way to use worked examples is to have students compare examples that

reach a right answer but are worked out in different ways. What is the same about each

solution? What is different? Why? (Rittle-Johnson & Star, 2007). Also, worked examples

should deal with one source of information at a time rather than having students move

between text passages, graphs, tables, and so on. The cognitive load will be too heavy for

beginners if they have to integrate many sources of information to make sense of the

worked examples (Marcus, Cooper, & Sweller, 1996).

Worked examples can serve as analogies or models for solving new problems. But

beware. Without explanations and coaching, novices may remember the surface features

Spencer

Grant/PhotoEdit,Inc

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

FIGURE 9.4

THE PROBLEM-SOLVING PROCESS

There are two paths to a solution. In the first, the correct schema is activated and the solution is

apparentthe new problem is an old one in disguise. But if no schema works, searching and testing

may provide a path to a solution.

Schema activated–I

solved this

before.

321

Succeed

Define Goals

and Represent

the Problem

What am I

being asked?

Explore Possible

Solutions

Any algorithms?

Would heuristics

help?

Anticipate

Consequences

and Act:

Try the Solution

Did it work?

Evaluate:

Reflect, look

back, or try again

No Schema

Fail

activatednever solved

before.

of a worked example or case instead of the deeper meaning or the structure. It is the

meaning or structure, not the surface similarities, that helps in solving new, analogous

problems (Gentner, Loewenstein, & Thompson, 2003; Goldstone & Day, 2012). Phil has

heard students complain that the test preparation problems in their math classes were

about boats and river currents, but the test asked about airplanes and wind speed. They

protested, There were no problems about boats on the test, and we never studied air-planes

in class! In fact, the problems on the test about airplanes were solved in exactly

the same way as the boat problems, but the students were focusing only on the surface

features. One way to overcome this tendency is to have students compare examples or

cases so they can develop a problem-solving schema that captures the common structure,

not the surface features, of the cases (Gentner et al., 2003).

How else might students develop the schemas they will need to represent problems

in a particular subject area? Mayer (1983) has recommended giving students practice in

the following: (1) recognizing and categorizing a variety of problem types; (2) represent-ing

problems, either concretely in pictures, symbols, or graphs, or in words; and (3) selecting

relevant and irrelevant information in problems.

The Results of Problem Representation. The problem representation stage of problem

solving has two main outcomes, as shown in Figure 9.4. If your representation of the

problem suggests an immediate solution, your task is done. In one sense, you have not

really solved a new problem; you have simply recognized the new problem as a dis-guised

version of an old problem that you already knew how to solve. This has been

called schema-driven problem solving. In terms of Figure 9.4, you can use the schema-activated

route and proceed directly to a solution.

But what if you have no existing way of solving the problem or your activated

schema fails? Time to search for a solution!

Searching for Possible Solution Strategies

In conducting your search for a solution, you have available two general kinds of proce-dures:

algorithmic and heuristic. Both of these are forms of procedural knowledge

(Schraw, 2006).

Schema-driven problem solving

Recognizing a problem as a

disguised version of an old

problem for which one already

has a solution

322 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

Algorithms. An algorithm is a step-by-step procedure for achieving a goal. It usually is

domain specific; that is, it is tied to a particular subject area. In solving a problem, if you

choose an appropriate algorithm (e.g., to find the arithmetic mean, you add all the scores,

then divide by the number of scores) and implement it properly, a right answer is guaran-teed.

Unfortunately, students often apply algorithms unsystematically, trying out one first,

and then another. They may even happen on the right answer, but not understand how

they got there, or they may forget the steps they used to find the answer. For some students,

applying algorithms haphazardly could be an indication that formal operational thinking

and the ability to work through a set of possibilities systematically (as described by Piaget)

is not yet developed. But many problems cannot be solved by algorithms. What then?

Heuristics. A heuristic is a general strategy that might lead to the right answer (Schoenfeld,

2011). Because many of lifes problems (careers, relationships, etc.) are not straightforward

and have ill-defined problem statements and no apparent algorithms, the discovery or

development of effective heuristics is important (Korf, 1999). Lets examine a few.

In means-ends analysis, the problem is divided into a number of intermediate goals

or subgoals, and then a means of solving each intermediate subgoal is figured out. For

example, writing a 20-page term paper can loom as an insurmountable problem for some

students. They would be better off breaking this task into several intermediate goals, such

as selecting a topic, locating sources of information, reading and organizing the informa-tion,

making an outline, and so on. As they attack a particular intermediate goal, they may

find that other goals arise. For example, locating information may require that they find

someone to refresh their memory about using the librarys databases. Keep in mind that

psychologists have yet to discover an effective heuristic for students who are just starting

their term paper the night before it is due.

Some problems lend themselves to a working-backward strategy. Using this heuristic,

you begin at the goal and move back to the unsolved initial problem. Working backward

is sometimes an effective heuristic for solving geometry proofs. It can also be a good way

to set intermediate deadlines (Lets see, if I have to submit this chapter in 4 weeks, I

should have a first draft finished by the 11th, and that means I better stop searching for

new references and start writing by. . . .).

Another useful heuristic is analogical thinking (Copi, 1961; Gentner et al., 2003), which

Algorithm Step-by-step

procedure for solving a problem;

prescription for solutions.

Heuristic General strategy used

in attempting to

solve problems.

Means-ends analysis Heuristic in

which a goal is divided into

subgoals.

Working-backward strategy

Heuristic in which one starts with

the goal and moves backward to

solve the problem.

Analogical thinking Heuristic in

which one limits the search for

solutions to situations that are

similar to the one at hand.

Verbalization Putting your

problem-solving plan and its

logic

into words.

limits your search for solutions to situations that have something in common with the one

you currently face. When submarines were first designed, for example, engineers had to

figure out how battleships could determine the presence and location of vessels hidden

in the depths of the sea. Studying how bats solve an analogous problem of navigating in

the dark led to the invention of sonar. Take note, however, that to use analogies effectively,

you must focus on meaning and not surface similarities, so focusing on bats appearance

would not have helped to solve the communication problem.

The possible analogies students bring to the classroom are bound to vary, based on

their experience and culture. For example, Zhe Chen and his colleagues wondered if post-secondary

students might use familiar folk talesone kind of cultural knowledgeas

analogies to solve problems (Chen, Mo, & Honomichl, 2004). That is just what happened.

Chinese students were better at solving a problem of weighing a statue because the prob-lem

was similar to their folk tale about how to weigh an elephant (by water displacement).

North American students were better at solving a problem of finding the way out of a cave

(leaving a trail) by using an analogy to Hansel and Gretel, a European folk tale commonly

told in North America.

Putting your problem-solving plan into words and giving reasons for selecting it can

lead to successful problem solving (Lee & Hutchinson, 1998). You may have discovered

the effectiveness of this verbalization process accidentally, when a solution popped into

your head as you were explaining a problem to someone else.

Anticipating, Acting, and Looking Back

After representing the problem and exploring possible solutions, the next step is to select

a solution and anticipate the consequences. For example, if you decide to solve the dam-aged

tomato problem by developing a tougher tomato, how will consumers react? If yo

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

take time to learn a new graphics program to enhance your term paper (and your grade),

will you still have enough time to finish the paper?

After you choose a solution strategy and implement it, evaluate the results by

checking for evidence that confirms or contradicts your solution. Many people tend to

stop working before they reach the best solution and simply accept an answer that

works in some cases. In mathematical problems, evaluating the answer might mean

applying a checking routine, such as adding to check the result of a subtraction prob-lem

or, in a long addition problem, adding the column from bottom to top instead of

top to bottom. Another possibility is estimating the answer. For example, if the com-putation

was 11 21, the answer should be around 200, because 10 20 is 200. A

student who reaches an answer of 2,311 or 32 or 562 should quickly realize these

answers cannot be correct. Estimating an answer is particularly important when stu-dents

rely on calculators or computers, because they cannot go back and spot an error

in the figures.

Factors That Hinder Problem Solving

Sometimes problem solving requires looking at things in new ways. People may miss

out on a good solution because they fixate on conventional uses for materials. This

difficulty is called functional fixedness (Duncker, 1945). In your everyday life, you may

often exhibit functional fixedness. Suppose a screw on a dresser-drawer handle is loose.

Will you spend 10 minutes searching for a screwdriver, or will you fix it with a ruler

edge or a dime?

Another kind of fixation that blocks effective problem solving is response set, getting

stuck on one way of representing a problem. Try this:

In each of the four matchstick arrangements below, move only one stick to change

the equation so that it represents a true equality such as V 5 V.

V 5 VII VI 5 XI XII 5 VII VI 5 II

You probably figured out how to solve the first example quite quickly. You simply move

one matchstick from the right side over to the left to make VI = VI. Examples two and

three can also be solved without too much difficulty by moving one stick to change the

V to an X or vice versa. But the fourth example (taken from Raudsepp & Haugh, 1977)

probably has you stumped. To solve this problem, you must change your response set or

switch schemas, because what has worked for the first three problems will not work this

time. The answer here lies in changing from Roman numerals to Arabic numbers and

using the concept of square root. By overcoming response set, you can move one match-stick

from the right to the left, across the top, to form the symbol for square root; the

solution reads v1 = I, which is simply the symbolic way of saying that the square root of

1 equals 1. Recently, a creative reader of this text emailed some other solutions. Jamaal

Allan, then a masters student at Pacific University, pointed out that you could use any of

the matchsticks to change the = sign to ?. Then, the last example would be V II or 5

does not equal 2, an accurate statement. He suggested that you also might move one

matchstick to change = to < or > and the statements would still be true (but not equali-ties

as specified in the problem above). Bill Wetta, a student at Ashland University, offered

another solution that used both Arabic and Roman numerals. You can move one match-stick

to make the first V an X. Then VI = II becomes XI 5 11, or 11 (in Roman numerals)

equals 11 (in Arabic numerals). Anita received another creative approach from Ray Part-low,

an educational psychology student. He noted, Simply remove a matchstick from the

V from the left-hand side, and place it directly on top of the I, getting II 5 II. Covering

one matchstick with another opens up a whole new set of possibilities! Can you come up

with any other solutions? Be creative!

Some Problems with Heuristics. We often apply heuristics automatically to make quick

judgments; that saves us time in everyday problem solving. The mind can react auto-matically

and instantaneously, but the price we often pay for this efficiency may be bad

problem solving, which can be costly. Making judgments by invoking stereotypes leads

even smart people to make dumb decisions. For example, we might use representativeness

Functional fixedness Inability to

use objects or tools in a new way.

Response set Rigidity; the

tendency to respond in the most

familiar way.

32

324 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

heuristics to make judgments about possibilities based on our prototypeswhat we think

is representative of a category. Consider this:

If I ask you whether a slim, short stranger who enjoys poetry is more likely to be a truck

driver or university classics professor, what would you say?

You might be tempted to answer based on your prototypes of truck drivers or professors.

But consider the odds. There are about 100 universities in Canada with perhaps an aver-age

of 2 or so classics professors per school. So, we have 200 professors. Say about 10%

are both short and slimthat is 20; and say half of those like poetrywe are left with 10.

Now suppose there are around 300 000 truck drivers in Canada. If only 1 in every 1000

of those truck drivers were short, slim, poetry lovers, we have 300 truck drivers who fit

the description. With 10 professors versus 300 truck drivers, it is 30 times more likely that

our stranger is a truck driver (Myers, 2005).

Teachers and students are busy people, and they often base their decisions on what

they have in their minds at the time. When judgments are based on the availability of

information in our memories, we are using the availability heuristic. If instances of events

come to mind easily, we think they are common occurrences, but that is not necessarily

the case; in fact, it is often wrong. People remember vivid stories and quickly come to

believe that such events are the norm, but again, they often are wrong. For example, you

may have been surprised to read in Chapter 4 that accelerating gifted students pace

through the grades does not undermine their social development. Data may not support

a judgment, but belief perseverance, or the tendency to hold on to our beliefs, even in the

face of contradictory evidence, may make us resist change.

The confirmation bias is the tendency to search for information that confirms our ideas

and beliefs: This arises from our eagerness to get a good solution. You have often heard

the saying Dont confuse me with the facts. This aphorism captures the essence of the

confirmation bias. Most people seek evidence that supports their ideas more readily than

they search for facts that might refute them. For example, once you decide to buy a certain

car, you are likely to notice reports about the good features of the car you chose, not the

good news about the cars you rejected. Our automatic use of heuristics to make judg-ments,

our eagerness to confirm what we like to believe, and our tendency to explain

away failure combine to generate overconfidence. Students usually are overconfident

about how fast they can get their papers written; it typically takes twice as long as they

estimate (Buehler, Griffin, & Ross, 1994). In spite of their underestimation of completion

time, they remain overly confident the next time around.

The Guidelines: Applying Problem Solving give some ideas for helping students

become good problem solvers.

Representativeness heuristic

Judging the likelihood of an

event based on how well the

events match your prototypeswhat

you think is representative

of the category.

Availability heuristic Judging the

likelihood of an event based on

what is available in your memory,

assuming those easily

remembered events are common.

Belief perseverance The

tendency to hold on to beliefs,

even in the face of contradictory

evidence.

Confirmation bias Seeking

information that confirms our

choices and beliefs, while

disconfirming evidence.

Expert Knowledge and Problem Solving

Most psychologists agree that effective problem solving is based on having an ample store

of knowledge about the problem area (Belland, 2011; Schoenfeld, 2011). To solve the

matchstick problem, for example, you had to understand Roman and Arabic numerals as

well as the concept of square root. You also had to know that the square root of 1 is 1.

Lets take a moment to examine this expert knowledge.

Knowing What Is Important. Experts know where to focus their attention. For example,

knowledgeable baseball fans pay attention to the position of the shortstop to learn if the

pitcher will throw a fastball, curveball, or slider. But those with little knowledge about

baseball may never notice the movements of the shortstop, unless a hit is headed toward

that part of the field (Bruning et al., 2011). In general, experts know what to pay attention

to when judging a performance or product such as Olympic diving or a prize-winning

chocolate cake. To nonexperts, most good dives or cakes look about the same, unless of

course they flop!

Memory for Patterns and Organization. The modern study of expertise began with

investigations of chess masters (Simon & Chase, 1973). Results indicated that masters can

quickly recognize about 50 000 different arrangements of chess pieces. They can look a

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES 325

Applying Problem Solving

GUIDELINES

Ask students if they are sure they understand the problem.

Examples

1. Can they separate relevant from irrelevant information?

2. Are they aware of the assumptions they are making?

3. Encourage them to visualize the problem by diagramming or

drawing it.

4. Ask them to explain the problem to someone else. What

would a good solution look like?

Encourage attempts to see the problem from different angles.

Examples

1. Suggest several different possibilities yourself, and then ask

students to offer some.

2. Give students practice in taking and defending different

points of view on an issue.

Let students do the thinking; do not just hand them solutions.

Examples

1. Offer individual problems as well as group problems, so that

each student has the chance to practice.

2. Give partial credit if students have good reasons for wrong

solutions to problems.

3. If students are stuck, resist the temptation to give too many

clues. Let them think about the problem overnight.

Help students develop systematic ways of considering

alternatives.

Examples

1. Think out loud as you solve problems.

2. Ask, What would happen if?

3. Keep a list of suggestions.

Teach heuristics.

Examples

1. Use analogies to solve the problem of limited parking in the

downtown area. How are other storage problems solved?

2. Use the working-backward strategy to plan a party.

For more resources on problem solving, see hawaii.edu/suremath/

home.html

one of these patterns for a few seconds and remember where every piece on the board

was placed. It is as though they have a vocabulary of 50 000 patterns. Michelene Chi

(1978) demonstrated that expert chess players in grades 3 through 8 had a similar ability

to remember chess piece arrangements. For all the masters, patterns of pieces are like

words. If you were shown any word from your vocabulary store for just a few seconds,

you would be able to remember every letter in the word in the right order (assuming you

could spell the word). But a series of letters arranged randomly is hard to remember, as

you saw in Chapter 8. An analogous situation holds for chess masters. When chess pieces

are placed on a board randomly, masters are no better than average players at remember-ing

the positions of the pieces. The masters memory is for patterns that make sense or

could occur in a game.

A similar phenomenon occurs in other fields. There may be an intuition about how

to solve a problem based on recognizing patterns and knowing the right moves for those

patterns. Experts in physics, for example, organize their knowledge around central prin-ciples

(e.g., Boyles or Newtons laws), whereas beginners organize their smaller amounts

of physics knowledge around the specific details stated in the problems (e.g., levers or

pulleys) (Ericsson, 1999; Fenton, 2007).

Procedural Knowledge. In addition to representing a problem very quickly, experts know

what to do next and can do it. They have a large store of productions or ifthen schemas

about what action to take in various situations. So, the steps of understanding the problem

and choosing a solution happen simultaneously and fairly automatically (Ericsson & Char-ness,

1999). Of course, this means that experts must have many, many schemas available.

A large part of becoming an expert is simply acquiring a great store of domain knowledge

or knowledge that is particular to a field (Alexander, 1992). To do this, you must encounter

many different kinds of problems in that field, observe others solving problems, and prac-tice

solving many yourself. Some estimates are that it takes 10 years or 10 000 hours of

deliberate, focused, sustained practice to become an expert in most fields (Ericsson, 2011

326 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

K. A. Ericsson & Charness, 1994; H. A. Simon, 1995).

Experts rich store of knowledge is elaborated and

well-practised, so that it is easy to retrieve from long-term

memory when needed (Anderson, 1993).

Planning and Monitoring. Experts spend more

time analyzing problems, drawing diagrams, break-ing

large problems down into subproblems, and

making plans. A novice might begin immediatelywriting

equations for a physics problem or drafting

the first paragraph of a paperbut experts plan out

the whole solution and often make the task simpler

in the process. As they work, experts monitor pro-gress,

so time is not lost pursuing dead ends or weak

ideas (Schunk, 2012).

So what can we conclude? Experts (1) know

EXPERT KNOWLEDGE A large part of becoming an expert is simply acquiring

a great store of domain knowledge, or knowledge that is particular to a field.

This surgeon has likely invested years of deliberate, focused, sustained practice

to become an expert.

where to focus their attention; (2) perceive large,

meaningful patterns in given information and are not

confused by surface features and details; (3) hold

more information in working and long-term memories,

in part because they have organized the information

into meaningful chunks and procedures; (4) take a

great deal of time to analyze a given problem; (5) have

automatic procedures for accomplishing pieces of the problem; and (6) are better at monitor-ing

their performance. When the area of problem solving is well defined, such as chess or

physics or computer programming, then these skills of expert problem solvers hold fairly

consistently. In these kinds of domains, even if students do not have the extensive back-ground

knowledge of experts, they can learn to approach the problem like an expert by

taking time to analyze the problem, focusing on key features, using the right schema, and

not trying to force old but inappropriate solutions on new problems (Belland, 2011). But

when the problem-solving area is less well defined and has fewer clear underlying principles,

such as problem solving in economics or psychology, then the differences between experts

and novices are not as clear-cut (Alexander, 1992).

CREATIVITY: WHAT IT IS AND WHY IT MATTERS

STOP & THINK Consider this student. He had severe dyslexiaa learning disability that made

reading and writing exceedingly difficult. He described himself as an underdog. In school, he

knew that if the reading assignment would take others an hour, he had to allow 2 or 3 hours.

He knew that he had to keep a list of all of his most frequently misspelled words to be able to

write at all. He spent hours alone in his room. Would you expect his writing to be creative? Why

or why not?

The person described in this Stop & Think is John Irving, celebrated author of what one

critic called wildly inventive novels such as The World According to Garp, The Cider

House Rules, and A Prayer for Owen Meany (Amabile, 2001). How do we explain his

amazing creativity? What is creativity?

Creativity is the ability to produce work that is original but still appropriate and use-Creativity

Imaginative, original

thinking or problem solving.

ful (Plucker, Beghetto, & Dow, 2004). Most psychologists agree that there is no such thing

as all-purpose creativity; people are creative in a particular area, as John Irving was in

writing fiction. But to be creative, the invention must be intended. An accidental spilling

of paint that produces a novel design is not creative unless the artist recognizes the poten-tial

of the accident or uses the spilling technique intentionally to create new works

(Weisberg, 1993). Although we frequently associate the arts with creativity, any subject

can be approached in a creative manner.

Levent

Konuk/Shutterstoc

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

Assessing Creativity

STOP & THINK How many uses can you list for a brick? Take a moment and brainstormwrite

down as many as you can.

Like the author John Irving, Paul Torrance had a learning disability. He became interested

in educational psychology when he was a high school English teacher (Neumeister &

Cramond, 2004). Torrance was known as the Father of Creativity. He developed two

types of creativity tests: verbal and graphic (Torrance, 1972; Torrance & Hall, 1980). In

the verbal test, you might be instructed to think up as many uses as possible for a brick

(as you did above) or asked how a particular toy might be changed to make it more fun.

On the graphic test, you might be given 30 circles and asked to create 30 different draw-ings,

with each drawing including at least one circle.

These creativity tests require divergent thinking, an important component of many

conceptions of creativity. Divergent thinking is the ability to propose many different ideas

or answers. Convergent thinking is the more common ability to identify only one answer.

Responses to all these creativity tasks are scored for originality, fluency, and flexibilitythree

aspects of divergent thinking. Originality is usually determined statistically. To be

original, a response must be given by fewer than 5 or 10 people out of every 100 who

take the test. Fluency is the number of different responses. Flexibility is generally meas-ured

by the number of different categories of responses. For instance, if you listed 20 uses

of a brick, but each was to build something, your fluency score might be high, but your

flexibility score would be low. Of the three measures, fluencythe number of responsesis

the best predictor of divergent thinking, but there is more to real-life creativity than

divergent thinking (Plucker et al., 2004).

A few possible indicators of creativity in your students are curiosity, concentration,

adaptability, high energy, humour (sometimes bizarre), independence, playfulness, non-conformity,

risk taking, attraction to the complex and mysterious, willingness to fantasize

and daydream, intolerance for boredom, and inventiveness (Sattler & Hoge, 2006).

OK, but So What: Why Does Creativity Matter?

We cannot read any news these days without feeling a bit depressed about the problems

facing the world. Economic problems, health problems, energy problems, political prob-lems,

violence, povertythe list goes on. Certainly todays and tomorrows complex

problems will require creative solutions. And creativity is important for an individuals

psychological, physical, social, and career success. In addition, evidence shows that

creativity and critical thinking are needed to prevent people or societies from being

trapped by ideology and dogma (Ambrose & Sternberg, 2012; Plucker et al., 2004). Alene

Starko (2014) described her recent visit to China, where educators all over that country

kept asking her how to help their students become more creative, flexible thinkers.

These Chinese students knock the top off the international tests, but a focus on master-ing

academics comes at a cost to creativity and critical thinking. In fact, many teachers

will tell you that the pressures of accountability and preparing their students for high-stakes

tests have forced teaching for student creativity and creative teaching out of the

classroom.

But we do not have to choose between understanding and creativity. Strategies that

support creativity also support deep understanding in school subjects, because deep

understanding comes from using the content in multiple ways and seeing different

implications of the knowledge. Creativity also supports intrinsic motivation, engagement,

and persistence in learning because creativity generates novelty and sparks interest

(Starko, 2014).

What Are the Sources of Creativity?

Researchers have studied cognitive processes, personality factors, motivational patterns,

and background experiences to explain creativity (Simonton, 2000). Teresa Amabile

Divergent thinking Coming up

with many possible solutions.

Convergent thinking Narrowing

possibilities to a single answer.

32

328 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

(1996, 2001) proposed a three-component model of creativity. Indi-viduals

or groups must have:

1. Domain-relevant skills, including talents and competencies that

are valuable for working in the domain, such as Michelangelos

skills in shaping stone, developed when he lived with a stonecut-ters

family as a child.

2. Creativity-relevant processes, including work habits and person-ality

traits such as John Irvings habit of working 10-hour days

to write and rewrite and rewrite until he perfected his stories.

3. Intrinsic task motivation, or a deep curiosity and fascination

with the task, can be greatly influenced by teachers and parents

who support autonomy, stimulate curiosity, encourage fantasy,

and provide challenge.

SOCIAL ACCEPTANCE OF CREATIVITY History is filled

with examples of creative breakthroughs rejected in their

time (for example, Galileos theory of the sun as the centre

of the solar system). Is todays society ready to welcome

creative contributions in the field of alternative energies?

Creativity and Cognition. Having a rich store of knowledge in an

area is the basis for creativity, but something more is needed. For

many problems, that something more is the ability to see things in

a new wayrestructuring the problem, which leads to a sudden

insight. Often this happens when a person has struggled with a

problem or project and then sets it aside for a while. Some psy-chologists

believe that time away allows for incubation, a kind of

unconscious working through the problem. Actually, it is more com-plex

than that. Incubation seems to help more on divergent thinking

tasks than on verbal or visual tasks. Also incubation is more helpful

when a longer preparation period precedes the individuals setting

the problem aside (Sio & Ormerod, 2009). Leaving the problem for

a time probably interrupts rigid ways of thinking so you can restruc-ture

your view of the situation and think more divergently (Gleit-man,

Fridlund, & Reisberg, 1999). Creativity requires extensive

knowledge, flexibility, and the continual reorganizing of ideas. And

we saw that motivation, persistence, and social support play impor-tant

roles as well.

Restructuring Conceiving of a

problem in a new or different

way.

Insight Sudden realization of a

solution; the ability to deal

effectively with novel situations.

Creativity and Diversity. As Dean Simonton said, even with years of research on creativ-ity,

Psychologists still have a long way to go before they come anywhere close to under-standing

creativity in women and minorities (2000, p. 156). Thus far, white males have

been the focus of creativity research and writing over the years. However, patterns of

creativity in other groups are complexsometimes matching and sometimes diverging

from patterns found in traditional research.

In another connection between creativity and culture, research suggests that being

on the outside of mainstream society, being bilingual, or being exposed to other cultures

might encourage creativity (Simonton, 2000). In fact, true innovators often break rules.

Creators have a desire to shake things up (Winner, 2000, p. 167). And even for those

who are not outside the mainstream, participation in multicultural experiences apparently

fosters creativity. Angela Ka-Yee Leung and her colleagues (2008; Maddux, Leung, Chui, &

Galinsky, 2009) reviewed theory and research, including experimental studies that exposed

participants to information and images about other cultures. The researchers concluded

that multicultural experiences support both creative processes, such as retrieving novel

or unconventional ideas from memory, and creative performance, such as generating

insightful solutions to problems. These effects are especially strong when people open

themselves up to divergent ideas and when the situation does not emphasize finding

quick, firm answers. Multicultural individuals are particularly willing to consider and build

on unfamiliar ideas, entertain conflicting alternatives, and make unlikely connections

between ideas (Leung & Chiu, 2010; Maddux & Galinsky, 2009). So even though your

students may not be able to travel to Tibet or Turkey, they still could become more crea-tive

problem solvers if they learned about different cultures.

Paul

Whitfield/DK

Image

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

Creativity in the Classroom

STOP & THINK Consider these three students described by Alene Starko (2014, p. 3):

In first grade, Michelle was given an outline of a giant sharks mouth on a worksheet

that asked, What will our fishy friend eat next? She dutifully coloured several fish and

boats, and then wrote the following explanation: Once there was a shark named

Peppy. One day he ate three fish, one jellyfish, and two boats. Before he ate the jel-lyfish,

he made a peanut butter and jellyfish sandwich.

At 19, Juan was homeless and a senior in high school. One cold evening, he thought

that a warm space inside the school would be a more appealing sleeping place than

any he could see. Getting into the building was no problem, but once he was inside a

motion detector would make him immediately detectable to the guard on the floor

below. Juan entered a storage room and carefully dislodged a pile of baseball bats. In

the ensuing commotion, he located a comfortable sleeping place. The guard attributed

the motion detectors outburst to the falling bats, and Juan slept until morning.

In 2003 Mark Zuckerberg hacked into Harvards Web site and downloaded student

ID photos into a Web site designed to compare student photos as hot or not. The

Web site lasted just a few days. Four months later he launched a new social networking

Web site called Thefacebook. The rest is history.

Are these students creative? What might teachers do to foster or to inhibit this creative think-ing?

All too often, in the crush of day-to-day classroom life, teachers stifle creative ideas

without realizing what they are doing. Teachers are in an excellent position to encourage or

discourage creativity through their acceptance or rejection of the unusual and imaginative.

In addition to encouraging creativity through everyday interactions with students,

teachers can try brainstorming. The basic tenet of brainstorming is to separate the process

of creating ideas from the process of evaluating them because evaluation often inhibits

creativity (Osborn, 1963). Evaluation, discussion, and criticism are postponed until all

possible suggestions have been made. In this way, one idea inspires others; people do not

withhold potentially creative solutions out of fear of criticism. Alene Starko (2014) gives

these rules for brainstorming:

1. No criticism of any ideas until all the ideas are on the table. This includes both verbal

and nonverbal criticism, so no eye-rolling or laughing.

2. Go for as many ideas as you can. Quantity may lead to quality as one idea inspires

another.

3. Feel free to hitchhike on other ideas. This means that it is okay to borrow elements

from ideas already on the table, or to make slight modifications of ideas already

suggested.

4. Encourage wild ideas. Impossible, totally unworkable ideas may lead someone to

think of other, more possible, more workable ideas. It is easier to take a wildly

imaginative bad idea and tone it down to fit the constraints of reality than it is to take

a boring bad idea and make it interesting enough to be worth thinking about.

Individuals as well as groups may benefit from brainstorming. In writing this book, for

example, Phil sometimes found it helpful to list all the different topics that could be cov-ered

in a chapter, then leave the list and return to it later to evaluate the ideas.

The Big C: Revolutionary Innovation

Ellen Winner (2000) describes the big-C creativity or innovation that establishes a new

field or revolutionizes an old one. Even child prodigies do not necessarily become adult

innovators. Prodigies have mastered well-established domains very early, but innovators

change the entire domain. Individuals who ultimately make creative breakthroughs tend

from their earliest days to be explorers, innovators, and tinkerers. Often, this adventurous-ness

is interpreted as insubordination, though more fortunate tinkerers receive from

teachers or peers some form of encouragement for their experimentation (Gardner, 1993,

329

Brainstorming Generating

ideas without stopping to

evaluate them

330 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

pp. 3233). What can parents and teachers do to encourage these tinkers and potential

creators? Winner (2000) lists four dangers to avoid:

1. Avoid pushing so hard that the childs intrinsic passion to master a field becomes a

craving for extrinsic rewards.

2. Avoid pushing so hard that the child later looks back on a missed childhood.

3. Avoid freezing the child into a safe, technically perfect way of performing that has

led to lavish rewards.

4. Be aware of the psychological wounds that can follow when the child who can per-form

perfectly becomes the forgotten adult who can do nothing more than continue

to perform perfectlywithout ever creating something new.

Finally, teachers and parents can encourage students with outstanding abilities and crea-tive

talents to give back to the society; service learning, discussed in Chapter 10, is one

opportunity.

The Guidelines: Applying and Encouraging Creativity, adapted from Fleith (2000)

and Sattler and Hoge (2006), describe other possibilities for encouraging creativity.

Applying and Encouraging Creativity

GUIDELINES

Accept and encourage divergent thinking.

Examples

1. During class discussion, ask Can anyone suggest a different

way of looking at this question?

2. Reinforce attempts at unusual solutions to problems, even if

the final product is not perfect.

3. Offer choices in topics for projects or modes of presentation

(written, oral, visual or graphic, using technology).

Tolerate dissent.

Examples

1. Ask students to support dissenting opinions.

2. Make sure nonconforming students receive an equal share of

classroom privileges and rewards.

Encourage students to trust their own judgment.

Examples

1. When students ask questions you think they can answer,

rephrase or clarify the questions and direct them back to

the students.

2. Give ungraded assignments from time to time.

Emphasize that everyone is capable of creativity in some form.

Examples

1. Avoid describing the feats of great artists or inventors as if

they were superhuman accomplishments.

2. Recognize creative efforts in each students work. Have a

separate grade for originality on some assignments.

Provide time, space, and materials to support creative projects.

Examples

1. Collect found materials for collages and creationsbuttons,

stones, shells, paper, fabric, beads, seeds, drawing

tools, claytry flea markets and friends for donations. Have

mirrors and pictures for drawing faces.

2. Make a well-lighted space available where children can work

on projects, leave them, and come back to finish them.

3. Follow up on memorable occasions (field trips, news

events, holidays) with opportunities to draw, write, or

make music.

Be a stimulus for creative thinking.

Examples

1. Use class brainstorming sessions whenever possible.

2. Model creative problem solving by suggesting unusual

solutions for class problems.

3. Encourage students to delay judging a particular suggestion

for solving a problem until all the possibilities have been

considered.

Capitalize on technology (Starko, 2014).

Examples

1. Have students use free apps such as Spider Scribe

(spiderscribe.net) to create visual maps of ideas and share

their ideas with others.

2. Spend the 5 minutes before lunch or at the end of class

creatively by having students practice divergent thinking

using Creative Genius on the Go on their iPhone, iPod, or

iPad.

3. Encourage students to create a mock Facebook page for a

literary or historical figure using Fakebook from Classtools.

net. Go to classtools.net/FB/home-page.

4. Use Wordle (wordle.net) or Tagxedo (tagxedo.com) to create

word clouds showing the frequency of words used in a

particular text or the students writing. See Figure 9.5 for a

word cloud of this chapter made with Wordle.

For more ideas, see ecap.crc.illinois.edu and search for creativity.

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

FIGURE 9.5

A WORD CLOUD OF THIS CHAPTER

In this word cloud, the frequency of the words in this chapter is indicated by the size of the word,

so you can see that students, problem, and strategies appear most often.

331

Source: Anita Woolfolk

We may not all be revolutionary in our creativity, but we all can be experts in one

areacritical thinking.

CRITICAL THINKING AND ARGUMENTATION

Critical thinking skills are useful in almost every life situationeven in evaluating the

media ads that constantly bombard us. When you see a group of gorgeous people extol-ling

the virtues of a particular brand of orange juice as they frolic in skimpy bathing suits,

you must decide if sex appeal is a relevant factor in choosing a fruit drink (remember

Pavlovian advertising from Chapter 7). A formal definition of critical thinking is the intel-lectually

disciplined process of actively and skillfully conceptualizing, applying, analyzing,

synthesizing, and/or evaluating information gathered from, or generated by, observation,

experience, reflection, reasoning, or communication, as a guide to belief and action

(Scriven & Paul, 2013). Table 9.3 describes the characteristics of a critical thinker.

Many educational psychologists believe that good thinking can and should be devel-oped

in school. One way to develop students thinking is to create a culture of thinking

in your classrooms (Perkins, Jay, & Tishman, 1993). This means that there is a spirit of

inquisitiveness and critical thinking, a respect for reasoning and creativity, and an expecta-tion

that students will learn to make and counter arguments based on evidence.

TABLE 9.3 What Is a Critical Thinker?

Assuming that critical thinking is reasonable, reflective thinking focused on deciding what to believe or

do, a critical thinker:

1. Is open minded and mindful of alternatives.

2. Tries to be well informed.

3. Judges well the credibility of

sources.

4. Identifies conclusions, reasons, and assumptions.

5. Judges well the quality of an argument, including the acceptability of its reasons, assumptions,

and evidence.

6. Can well develop and defend a reasonable position.

7. Asks appropriate clarifying questions.

8. Formulates plausible hypotheses; plans experiments well.

9. Defines terms in a way appropriate for the context.

10. Draws conclusions when warranted, but with caution.

11. Integrates all items in this list when deciding what to believe or do.

Source: Based on Robert H. Ennis: http://faculty.ed.uiuc.edu/rhennis/index.html, Pearson Educatio

332 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

FIGURE 9.6

PAUL AND ELDERS MODEL OF CRITICAL THINKING

Critical thinkers routinely apply the intellectual standards to the elements of reasoning to

develop

intellectual traits.

THE STANDARDS

clarity

accuracy

relevance

logic

breadth

precision

significance

completeness

fairness

depth

THE ELEMENTS

purposes

questions

as we

learn to

develop

points of view

information

inferences

concepts

implications

assumptions

must be

applied to

INTELLECTUAL TRAITS

intellectual humility

intellectual autonomy

intellectual integrity

intellectual courage

intellectual perseverance

intellectual empathy

fairmindedness

confidence in reason

Source: Paul, R., & Elder, L. (2012). Critical Thinking: Tools for Taking Charge of Your Learning and Your Life (3rd ed., p. 58). Upper Saddle

River, NJ: Pearson. Reprinted by permission of Pearson, Inc, Pearson Education.

One Model of Critical Thinking: Paul and Elder

What is involved in critical thinking? Richard Paul and Linda Elder (2014; Elder & Paul,

2012) suggest the model in Figure 9.6 as a way of describing what critical thinkers do. As

you can see, the centre of critical thinking is reasoning, which is drawing conclusions

based on reasons. When we reason, we have a purpose and a point of view. We reason

based on certain assumptions that lead to implications for our conclusions. We use infor-mation

(data, facts, experiences, etc.) to make inferences and judgments based on key

concepts or ideas, all leading to answers to the main problem or question indicated in

our original purpose. But to reason wellto think criticallywe should apply standards

such as clarity, accuracy, logic, and fairness, as indicated in Figure 9.6. With practice in

clear, accurate, logical (etc.) reasoning, we develop intellectual traits such as humility,

integrity, perseverance, and confidence.

So how would you develop critical thinking in your classes? No matter what approach

you use to develop critical thinking, it is important to follow up with additional practice.

One lesson is not enough. For example, if your class examined a particular historical

document to determine if it reflected bias or propaganda, you should follow up by analyz-ing

other written historical documents, contemporary advertisements, or news stories.

Unless thinking skills become overlearned and relatively automatic, they are not likely to

be transferred to new situations (Mayer & Wittrock, 2006). Instead, students will use these

skills only to complete the lesson in social studies, not to evaluate the claims made by

friends, politicians, car manufacturers, or diet plans

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

Applying Critical Thinking in Specific Subjects

The characteristics of critical thinkers in Table 9.3 would be useful in any subject. But some

critical thinking skills are specific to a particular subject. For example, to teach history,

Jeffrey Nokes and his colleagues investigated (1) using traditional texts versus multiple

readings and (2) direct teaching of critical thinking skills versus no direct teaching of criti-cal

thinking skills (Nokes, Dole, & Hacker, 2007). The multiple texts included historical

fiction, excerpts from speeches, government documents, photographs, charts and historical

data, and short sections from texts. The history critical thinking skills taught were:

Sourcing: Looking at the source of the document before reading and using that infor-mation

to help interpret and make inferences about the reading. Is the source biased?

Can I trust it?

Corroboration: Making connections between the information in different texts and

noting similarities and contradictions.

Contextualization: Understanding the time, place, people, and culture that is the

context for the event, with all the political and social forces that might be operating.

Students who learned with multiple texts instead of traditional textbooks actually

learned more history content. Also, students were able to learn and apply two of the

three critical thinking skills, sourcing and corroboration, when they were directly taught

how to use the skills. Contextualization proved more difficult, perhaps because the

students lacked the background knowledge to fill in contextual information. So critical

thinking for specific subjects can be taught along with the subject. But as you can see

in the Point/Counterpoint, educators do not agree about the best way to foster critical

thinking in schools.

Argumentation

The ability to construct and support a position is essential in science, politics, persuasive

writing, and critical thinking, to name just a few areas. The heart of argumentation (the

process of constructing and critiquing arguments, and debating claims) is supporting your

position with evidence and understanding and then refuting your opponents claims and

evidence. Children are not skilled at argumentation, adolescents are a bit better, and adults

are better still, but not perfect. Children do not pay very much attention to the claims and

evidence of the other person in the debate. Adolescents understand that their opponent

in a debate has a different position, but they tend to spend much more time presenting

their own position than they do trying to understand and critique their opponents claims.

It is as if the adolescents believe winning an argument means making a better presenta-tion,

but they do not appreciate the need to understand and weaken the opponents claims

(Kuhn & Dean, 2004; Nussbaum, 2011).

Children and adolescents focus more on their own positions because it is too demand-ing

to remember and process both their own and their opponents claims and evidence

at the same timethe cognitive load is just too much. In addition, argumentation skills

are not natural. They take both time and instruction to learn (Kuhn, Goh, Iordanou, &

Shaenfield, 2008; Udell, 2007).

But what has to be learned? To make a case while understanding and refuting the

opponents case, you must be aware of what you are saying, what your opponent is say-ing,

and how to refute your opponents claims. This takes planning, evaluating how the

plan is going, reflecting on what the opponent has said, and changing strategies as

neededin other words, metacognitive knowledge and skills for argumentation. Deanna

Kuhn and her colleagues (2008) designed a process for developing metacognitive argu-mentation

skills. They presented a grade 6 class with the following dilemma.

The Costa family has moved to the edge of town from far away Greece with their

11-year-old son Nick. Nick was a good student and soccer player back home in Greece.

Nicks parents have decided that in this new place, they want to keep Nick at home with

them, and not have him be at the school with the other children. The family speaks only

Greek, and they think Nick will do better if he sticks to his familys language and doesnt

try to learn English. They say they can teach him everything he needs at home. What

333

Argumentation The process of

debating a claim with someone

else

334 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

POINT/

COUNTERPOINT

Should Schools Teach Critical Thinking and

Problem Solving?

The question of whether schools should focus on process or content, problem-solving skills or core knowledge,

higher-order thinking skills or academic information has been debated for years. Some educators suggest that

students must be taught how to think and solve problems, while other educators assert that students cannot

learn to think in the abstract. They must be thinking about somethingsome content. Should teachers focus

on knowledge or thinking?

Problem solving and higher-order thinking can and

should be taught. An article in the April, 28, 1995, issue of

the Chronicle of Higher Education makes this claim:

Critical thinking is at the heart of effective reading,

writing, speaking, and listening. It enables us to link

together mastery of content with such diverse goals

as self-esteem, self-discipline, multicultural educa-tion,

effective cooperative learning, and problem

solving. It enables all instructors and administrators

to raise the level of their own teaching and thinking.

(Based on Chronicle of Higher Education, April, 28,

1995, p. A-71., Pearson Education.)

Closer to home for you, Peter Facione (2011) claims that critical

thinking is related to GPA in college or university and to reading

comprehension. How can students learn to think critically? Some

educators recommend teaching thinking skills directly with widely

used techniques such as the Productive Thinking Program or

CoRT (Cognitive Research Trust). Other researchers argue that

learning computer programming languages will improve stu-dents

minds and teach them how to think logically. Finally,

because expert readers automatically apply certain metacogni-tive

strategies, many educators and psychologists recommend

directly teaching novice or poor readers how to apply these strat-egies.

Michael Pressleys Good Strategy User model (Pressley &

Harris, 2006) and Palincsar and Browns (1984) reciprocal teaching

approach are successful examples of direct teaching of metacog-nitive

skills. Research on these approaches generally shows

improvements in achievement and comprehension for students

of all ages who participate (Pressley & Harris, 2006; Rosenshine &

Meister, 1994).

Thinking and problem-solving skills do not transfer. E. D.

Hirsch, a vocal critic of critical thinking programs, writes:

But whether such direct instruction of critical thinking

or self-monitoring does in fact improve performance

is a subject of debate in the research community. For

instance, the research regarding critical thinking is

not reassuring. Instruction in critical thinking has

been going on in several countries for over a hun-dred

years. Yet researchers found that students from

nations as varied as Israel, Germany, Australia, the

Philippines, and the United States, including those

who have been taught critical thinking continue to

fall into logical fallacies. (Hirsch, 1996, p. 136)

The CoRT program has been used in over 5000 classrooms

in 10 nations. But Polson and Jeffries (1985) report that

after 10 years of widespread use we have no adequate

evidence concerning the effectiveness of the program (p. 445).

In addition, Mayer and Wittrock (1996) note that field studies of

problem solving in real situations show that people often fail to

apply the mathematical problem-solving approaches they learn

in school to actual problems encountered in the grocery store

or home.

Even though educators have been more successful in

teaching metacognitive skills, critics still caution that there are

times when such teaching hinders rather than helps learning.

Robert Siegler (1993) suggests that teaching self-monitoring

strategies to low-achieving students can interfere with the stu-dents

development of adaptive strategies. Forcing students to

use the strategies of experts may put too much burden on work-ing

memory as the students struggle to use an unfamiliar strat-egy

and miss the meaning or content of the lesson. For example,

rather than teach students strategies for figuring out words from

context, it may be helpful for students to focus on learning more

vocabulary words.

BEWARE OF EITHER/OR

One clear message from current research on learning is that both

subject-specific knowledge and learning strategies are important.

Students today need to be critical consumers of all kinds of

knowledge, but critical thinking alone is not enough. Students

need the knowledge, vocabulary, and concepts to understand

what they are reading, seeing, and hearing. The best teachers can

teach math content and how to learn math at the same time or

can teach history and how to critically assess history sources.

POINT

.

Kawing921/Shutterstock

COUNTERPOINT

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

should happen? Is it okay for the Costa family to live in the town but keep Nick at home,

or should they be required to send their son to the town school like all the other

families do? (p. 1313)

Based on their initial position on the dilemma, the 28 students in the class were divided

into two groupsNick should go to school or Nick should be taught at home. These

two groups were divided again into same-gender pairs and all the Nick should go to

school pairs moved to a room next door to their class. For about 25 minutes, each pair

from one side debated a pair in the other room using instant messaging (IM). Later in

the week the process was repeated, but with different pairs debating. In all, there were

seven IM debates, so every go to school pair debated every stay home pair over sev-eral

weeks. After four of the seven sessions, the pairs were given a transcript of the

dialogue from their last debate, along with worksheets that scaffolded their reflection on

their own arguments or the arguments of their opponents. The students evaluated their

arguments and tried to improve them, with some adults coaching. These reflective ses-sions

were repeated three times.

Next, there was a showdown debatethe entire go to school team debated the

entire stay home team via one computer per team and a smart board. For this debate,

half of each team prepared as experts on their position and half as experts on the oppo-nents

arguments. After winter break and again after spring break, the whole process was

repeated with new dilemmas.

You can see that the study employed three techniques, supported by technology,

to help students become more metacognitive about argumentation. First, they had to

work in pairs to collaborate and agree on each communication with the opposing

pair. Second, the researchers provided the pairs with transcripts of parts of their

dialogue with the opponents so the partners could reflect on the discussions. Third,

the dialogues were conducted via IM, so the pairs had a permanent record of the

discussion.

So what happened? The pairs, IM, and reflection strategies were successful for most

students in helping them take into account the opponents position and create strategies

for rebutting the opponents arguments. Working in pairs seemed to be especially helpful.

When adolescents and even adults work alone, they often do not create effective coun-terarguments

and rebuttals (Kuhn & Franklin, 2006).

TEACHING FOR TRANSFER

STOP & THINK Think back for a moment to a class in one of your high school subjects that

you have not studied since. Imagine the teacher, the room, the textbook. Now remember what

you actually learned in class. If it was a science class, what were some of the formulas you

learned? Oxidation reduction? Boyles law?

If you are like most of us, you may remember that you learned these things, but you

will not be quite sure exactly what you learned. Were those hours wasted? This ques-tion

relates to the important topic of learning transfer. Lets begin with a definition

of transfer.

Whenever something previously learned influences current learning or when solving

an earlier problem affects how you solve a new problem, transfer has occurred. Erik De

Corte (2003) calls transfer the productive use of cognitive tools and

motivations

(p. 142),

and Chi and VanLehn (2012) describe transfer as the ability of students to treat a new

situation, problem, concept, or challenge as similar to one they have experienced before.

So transfer is doing something new (productive), not just reproducing a previous appli-cation

of the tools. If students learn a mathematical principle in one class and use it to

solve a physics problem days or weeks later in another class, then transfer has taken

place. However, the effect of past learning on present learning is not always positive.

335

Transfer Influence of previously

learned material on new material;

the productive (not reproductive)

uses of cognitive tools and

motivations

336 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

Functional fixedness and response set (described earlier in this chapter) are examples

of negative transfer because they are attempts to apply familiar but inappropriate strate-gies

to a new situation.

Transfer has several dimensions (Barnett & Ceci, 2002). You can transfer learning

across subjects (math skills used in science problems), across physical contexts (learned

in school, used on the job), across social contexts (learned alone, used with your family

or team), across time periods (learned in university, used months or years later), across

functions (learned for academics, used for hobbies and recreation), and across modalities

(learned from watching home improvement videos, used to discuss ideas for a patio with

a landscape architect). So transfer can refer to many different examples of applying knowl-edge

and skills beyond where, when, and how you learned them.

The Many Views of Transfer

Transfer has been a focus of research in educational psychology for over 100 years. After

all, the productive use of knowledge, skills, and motivations across a lifetime is a funda-mental

goal of education (Goldstone & Day, 2012; Shaffer, 2010). Early work focused on

specific transfer of skills and the general transfer of mental discipline gained from study-ing

rigorous subjects such as Greek or mathematics. But in 1924, E. L. Thorndike demon-strated

that no mental discipline benefit is derived from learning Greek. Learning Greek

just helps you learn more Greek. So, thanks to Thorndike, you were not required to take

Greek in high school.

More recently, researchers have distinguished between the automatic, direct use

of skills such as reading or writing in everyday applications and the thoughtful transfer

of knowledge and strategies to arrive at creative solutions to problems (Bereiter, 1995;

Bransford & Schwartz, 1999). Automatic transfer probably benefits from practice in

different situations, but thoughtful transfer requires more than practice. Michelene Chi

and Kurt VanLehn (2012) describe thoughtful transfer as involving two processesinitial

learning and reusing or applying what was learned. For thoughtful transfer to

succeed, students must first actually learn the underlying

principle or concept, not just the surface procedure or algo-rithm.

So, essential to thoughtful transfer in the initial learn-ing

stage is mindful abstraction, which is the deliberate

identification of a principle, main idea, strategy, or procedure

that is not tied to one specific problem or situation but could

apply to many. Such an abstraction becomes part of your

metacognitive knowledge, available to guide future learning

and problem solving. This may remind you of our discussion

in Chapter 8 about how the way you learn something in the

first place (through deeper processing) affects how well you

remember it later. Bransford and Schwartz (1999) added

another keya resource-rich environment that supports pro-ductive,

appropriate transfer. Table 9.4 summarizes the types

of transfer.

Teaching for Positive Transfer

Here is a great perspective on transfer from David Perkins and

Gavriel Salomon (2012):

HIGHER-LEVEL TRANSFER Students will be more likely to

transfer knowledge to new situations if they have been actively

involved in the learning process. They should be encouraged to

form abstractions that they will apply later, so the students know

transfer is an important goal.

Schools are supposed to be stopovers in life, not ends in

themselves. The information, skills, and understandings

they offer are knowledge-to-go, not just to use on site. To

be sure, often Mondays topics most conspicuously serve

the Tuesday problem set, the Friday quiz, or the exam at

the end of the year. However, in principle those topics are

an investment toward thriving in family, civic, cultural, and

professional lives. (p. 248)
Cindy

Charles/PhotoEdit,Inc

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

TABLE 9.4 Kinds of Transfer

DIRECT APPLICATION

Definition Automatic transfer of highly

practised skill

Key Conditions Extensive practice

Variety of settings and

conditions

Examples

Overlearning to automaticity

Driving many different cars

337

PREPARATION FOR FUTURE LEARNING

Conscious application of abstract

knowledge to a new situation

Productive use of cognitive tools and

motivations

Mindful focus on abstracting a principle,

main idea, or procedure that can be

used in many situations

Learning in powerful teachinglearning

environments

Applying KWL or READS strategies

Finding your gate in an airport Applying procedures from math in

designing a page layout for the school

newspaper

Years of research and experience show that students will not always take advantage

of knowledge-to-go. They may (seem to) learn new concepts, problem-solving procedures,

and learning strategies Monday, but they may not use them for the year-end exam or even

Friday unless prompted or guided. For example, studies of real-world mathematics show

that people do not always apply math procedures learned in school to solve practical

problems in their homes or at grocery stores (Lave, 1988; Lave & Wenger, 1991). This hap-pens

because learning is situatedtied to specific situations. Because knowledge is

learned as a tool to solve particular problems, we may not realize that the knowledge is

relevant when we encounter a problem that seems different, at least on the surface

(Driscoll, 2005; Singley & Anderson, 1989). How can you make sure your students will

use what they learn, even when situations change?

What Is Worth Learning? First, you must answer the question What is worth learning?

The learning of basic skills such as reading, writing, computing, cooperating, and speak-ing

will definitely transfer to other situations, because these skills are necessary for later

work both in and out of schoolwriting job applications, reading novels, paying bills,

working on a team, locating and evaluating health care services, among others. All later

learning depends on positive transfer of these basic skills to new situations.

Teachers must also be aware of what the future is likely to hold for their students,

both as a group and as individuals. What will society require of them as adults? As chil-dren,

we studied nothing about computers; now we spend hours at our Macs each day

and Phil even designs software for researching how students learn. Phil also learned to

use a slide rule. Now, calculators and computers have made this skill obsolete. We were

all encouraged to take advanced math and science instead of typing in high school. Those

were great classes, but we still struggle with keyboardingwho knew? Undoubtedly,

changes as extreme and unpredictable as these await the students you will teach. For this

reason, the general transfer of principles, attitudes, learning strategies, self-motivation,

time management skills, and problem solving will be just as important for your students

as the specific transfer of basic skills.

How Can Teachers Help? For basic skills, greater transfer can also be ensured by

overlearning, practising a skill

past the point of mastery

. Many of the basic facts stu-dents

learn in elementary school, such as the multiplication tables, are traditionally

overlearned. Overlearning helps students develop automated basic skills, as we saw in

Chapter 8.

Overlearning Practising a skill

past the point of mastery

338 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

For higher-level transfer, students must first learn and understand. Students will be

more likely to transfer knowledge to new situations if they have been actively involved

in the learning process. Strategies include having students compare and contrast two

examples, then identify the underlying principles; asking student to explain to themselves

or each other the worked examples provided by the teacher; or identify for each step in

a problem solution the underlying principle at work (Chi & VanLehn, 2012). Students

should be encouraged to form abstractions that they will apply later, so they know trans-fer

is an important goal. It also helps if students form deep connections between the new

knowledge and their existing structures of knowledge as well as connections to their

everyday experiences (Perkins & Salomon, 2012; Pugh & Phillips, 2011). Erik De Corte

(2003) believes that teachers support transfer, the productive use of cognitive tools and

motivations, when they create powerful teachinglearning environments using these

design principles:

The environments should support constructive learning processes in all students.

The environments should encourage the development of student self-regulation, so

that teachers gradually give over more and more responsibilities to the students.

Learning should involve interaction and collaboration.

Learners should deal with problems that have personal meaning for them, that are

similar to those they will face in the future.

The classroom culture should encourage students to become aware of and develop

their cognitive and motivational processes. To be productive users of these tools, stu-dents

must know about and value them.

Chapters 10 to 13 delve in depth about how to support constructive learning, motiva-tion,

self-regulation, collaboration, and self-awareness in all students.

One last kind of transfer is especially important for studentsthe transfer of the

learning strategies we encountered earlier. Learning strategies are meant to be applied

across a wide range of situations.

Stages of Transfer for Strategies. Gary Phye (1992, 2001; Phye & Sanders, 1994)

describes three stages in developing strategic transfer. In the acquisition phase, stu-dents

should not only receive instruction about a strategy and how to use it, but also

rehearse the strategy and practice being aware of when and how they are using it. In

the retention phase, more practice with feedback helps students hone their strategy

use. In the transfer phase, students should be given new problems that they can solve

with the same strategy, even though the problems appear different on the surface. To

enhance motivation, teachers should point out to students how using the strategy will

help them solve many problems and accomplish different tasks. These steps help build

both procedural and self-regulatory knowledgehow to use the strategy as well as

when and why.

For all students, there is a positive relationship between using learning strategies and

academic gains such as high school GPA and retention in university (Robbins, Le, & Lau-ver,

2005; Winne 2013). Some students will learn productive strategies on their own, but

all students can benefit from direct teaching, modelling, and practice of learning strategies

and study skills. This is one important way to prepare all of your students for the future.

Newly mastered concepts, principles, and strategies must be applied in a wide variety of

situations and with many types of problems (Z. Chen & Mo, 2004). Positive transfer is

encouraged when skills are practised under authentic conditions, similar to those that will

exist when the skills are needed later. Students can learn to write by corresponding with

email pen pals in other countries. They can learn historical research methods by studying

their own family history. Some of these applications should involve complex, ill-defined,

unstructured problems, because many of the problems to be faced in later life, both in

school and out, will not come to students complete with instructions. The Guidelines:

Family and Community Partnerships give ideas for enlisting the support of families in

encouraging transfer

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES 339

Promoting Transfer

GUIDELINES FAMILY AND COMMUNITY PARTNERSHIPS

Keep families informed about their childs curriculum so they

can support learning.

Examples

1. At the beginning of units or major projects, send aletter

summarizing the key goals, a few of the major assignments,

and some common problems students have in learning the

material for that unit.

2. Ask parents for suggestions about how their childs interests

could be connected to the curriculum topics.

3. Invite parents to school for an evening of strategy learning.

Have the students teach their family members one of the

strategies they have learned in school.

Give families ideas for how they might encourage their

children to practice, extend, or apply learning from school.

Examples

1. To extend writing, ask parents to encourage their children to

write letters or emails to companies or civic organizations

asking for information or free products. Provide a shell letter

form for structure and ideas, and include addresses of

companies that provide free samples or information.

2. Ask family members to include their children in some

projects that require measurement, halving or doubling

recipes, or estimating costs.

3. Suggest that students work with grandparents to do

a family memory book. Combine historical research and

writing.

Show connections between learning in school and life

outside school.

Examples

1. Ask families to talk about and show how they use the skills

their children are learning in their jobs, hobbies, or

community involvement projects.

2. Ask family members to come to class to demonstrate how

they use reading, writing, science, math, or other knowledge

in their work.

Make families partners in practising learning strategies.

Examples

1. Focus on one learning strategy at a time. Ask families to

simply remind their children to use a particular strategy with

homework that week.

2. Develop alending library of books and videotapes to teach

families about learning strategies.

3. Give parents a copy of the Guidelines: Becoming an Expert

Student, rewritten for your grade level.

. SUMMARY

METACOGNITION (PP. 304307)

What are the three metacognitive skills? The three metacogni-tive

skills used to regulate thinking and learning are planning,

monitoring, and evaluating. Planning involves deciding how

much time to give to a task, which strategies to use, how to

start, and so on. Monitoring is the real-time awareness of how

Im doing. Evaluating involves making judgments about the

processes and outcomes of thinking and learning and acting on

those judgments.

What are some sources of individual differences in meta-cognition?

Individual differences in metacognition may result

from different paces of development (maturation) or biologi-cal

differences among learners. For example, young students

may not be able to understand a lessons purpose as well as

older students.

How can teachers help students develop metacognitive

knowledge and skills? With younger students, teachers can

help students look inside to identify what they do to read,

write, or learn better. Systems such as KWL can help, if teach-ers

demonstrate, explain, and model the strategy. For older

students, teachers can build self-reflective questions into assign-ments

and learning materials.

LEARNING STRATEGIES

(PP. 307315)

What are learning strategies?

Learning strategies are a special kind

of procedural knowledgeknowing

how to do something. A strategy for

learning might include mnemonics to

remember key terms, skimming to identify the organization, and

then writing answers to possible essay questions. Use of strate-gies

and tactics reflects metacognitive knowledge.

What key functions do learning strategies play? Learning

strategies help students become cognitively engagedfocus

attention on the relevant, important aspects of the material.

Second, they encourage students to invest effort, make con-nections,

elaborate, translate, organize, and reorganize to

think and process deeply; the greater the practice and process-ing,

the stronger the learning. Finally, strategies help students

regulate and monitor their own learningkeep track of what

is making sense and notice when a new approach is needed.

Describe some procedures for developing learning strategies.

Expose students to a number of different strategies, not only

Liubomir/Shutterstoc

340 PART 2 LEARNING AND MOTIVATION

general learning strategies but also very specific tactics, such as

the graphic strategies. Teach conditional knowledge about when,

where, and why to use various strategies. Develop motivation to

use the strategies and tactics by showing students how their learn-ing

and performance can be improved. Provide direct instruction

in content knowledge needed to use the strategies.

When will students apply learning strategies? If they have appro-priate

strategies, students will apply them if they are faced with a

task that requires good strategies, value doing well on that task,

think the effort to apply the strategies will be worthwhile, and

believe that they can succeed using the strategies. Also, to apply

deep processing strategies, students must assume that knowledge

is complex and takes time to learn and that learning requires their

own active efforts.

PROBLEM SOLVING (PP. 315326)

What is problem solving? Problem solving is both general and

domain specific. Also, problems can range from well structured

to ill structured, depending on how clear-cut the goal is and how

much structure is provided for solving the problem. General prob-lem-solving

strategies usually include the steps of identifying the

problem, setting goals, exploring possible solutions and conse-quences,

acting, and finally evaluating the outcome. Both general

and specific problem solving are valuable and necessary.

Why is the representation stage of problem solving so important?

To represent the problem accurately, you must understand both

the whole problem and its discrete elements. Schema training may

improve this ability. The problem-solving process follows entirely

different paths, depending on what representation and goal are

chosen. If your representation of the problem suggests an immediate

solution, the task is done; the new problem is recognized as a dis-guised

version of an old problem with a clear solution. But if there

is no existing way of solving the problem or if the activated schema

fails, then students must search for a solution. The application of

algorithms and heuristicssuch as means-ends analysis, working

backward, analogical thinking, and verbalizationmay help students

solve problems.

Describe factors that can interfere with problem solving. Factors

that hinder problem solving include functional fixedness or rigidity

(response set). These disallow the flexibility needed to represent

problems accurately and to have insight into solutions. Also, as we

make decisions and judgments, we may overlook important infor-mation

because we base judgments on what seems representative

of a category (representativeness heuristic) or what is available in

memory (availability heuristic), then pay attention only to informa-tion

that confirms our choices (confirmation bias) so that we hold

on to beliefs, even in the face of contradictory evidence (belief

perseverance).

What are the differences between expert and novice knowl-edge

in a given area? Expert problem solvers have a rich store

of declarative, procedural, and conditional knowledge. They

organize this knowledge around general principles or patterns

that apply to large classes of problems. They work faster, remem-ber

relevant information, and monitor their progress better than

novices.

CREATIVITY: WHAT IT IS AND WHY IT MATTERS (PP. 326331)

What is creativity, and how is it assessed? Creativity is a process

that involves independently restructuring problems to see things

in new, imaginative ways. Creativity is difficult to measure, but

tests of divergent thinking can assess originality, fluency, and flex-ibility.

Originality is usually determined statistically. To be original,

a response must be given by fewer than 5 or 10 people out of

every 100 who take the test. Fluency is the number of different

responses. The number of different categories of responses meas-ures

flexibility.

What can teachers do to support creativity in the classroom?

Multicultural experiences appear to help students think flexibly and

creatively. Teachers can encourage creativity in their interactions

with students by accepting unusual, imaginative answers; model-ling

divergent thinking; using brainstorming; and tolerating dissent.

CRITICAL THINKING AND ARGUMENTATION (PP. 331335)

What is critical thinking? Critical thinking skills include defining

and clarifying the problem, making judgments about the consist-ency

and adequacy of the information related to a problem, and

drawing conclusions. No matter what approach you use to develop

critical thinking, it is important to follow up activities with additional

practice. One lesson is not enoughoverlearning will help stu-dents

use critical thinking in their own lives.

What is argumentation? The heart of argumentation (the process

of debating a claim with someone else) is supporting your position

with evidence and understanding, and then refuting your oppo-nents

claims and evidence. Argumentation skills are not natural.

They take both time and instruction to learn. It is especially difficult

for children and adolescents to pay attention to, understand, and

refute the opponents position with evidence.

TEACHING FOR TRANSFER (PP. 335339)

What is transfer? Transfer occurs when a rule, fact, or skill learned

in one situation is applied in another situation; for example, apply-ing

rules of punctuation to write a job application letter. Transfer

also involves applying to new problems the principles learned in

other, often dissimilar, situations.

What are some dimensions of transfer? Information can be trans-ferred

across a variety of contexts. Some examples include transfer

from one subject to another, one physical location to another, or

one function to another. These types of transfer make it possible to

use skills developed in one area for many other tasks.

Distinguish between automatic and mindful, intentional trans-fer.

Spontaneous application of well-learned knowledge and skills

is automatic transfer. Mindful, intentional transfer involves reflec-tion

and deliberate application of abstract knowledge to new situ-ations.

Learning environments should support active constructive

learning, self-regulation, collaboration, and awareness of cognitive

tools and motivational processes. In addition, students should

deal with problems that have meaning in their lives. In addition,

teachers can help students transfer learning strategies by teach-ing

strategies directly, providing practice with feedback, and then

expanding the application of the strategies to new and unfamiliar

situations

CHAPTER 9 COMPLEX COGNITIVE PROCESSES

. what would they do?

TEACHERS CASEBOOK: Uncritical Thinking

Here is how some practising teachers would help students learn to

critically evaluate the information they find on the internet.

JOHN BALDASSARRE

Formerly from Archbishop Oscar Romero High School, Edmonton, AB

When assigning a research paper, I find it extremely valuable to

start the process early in the year by presenting students with a

website that features afake news story that appears to be real. I ask

students to begin to discuss and evaluate the story that is presented

and to discuss any similar events that are taking place in the world

at the time. After a lengthy discussion, I reveal to the students the

fact that the website and the news story are fake and begin to show

them how to establish the validity of a website or author. I ask stu-dents

to consider the following basic questions:

Who is the author of the news story or website, and what is

his or her background? Is there anything on the site that could

bias the information?

What is the purpose of the website? Is it affiliated with any

other sites (political parties, social action groups, etc.)? Is it as-sociated

with a specific domain, or is it a personal site?

How active and recent is the website?

Is the content based on opinions or on studies and/or articles?

Can those studies or articles be accessed?

Can you find the same information stated on other websites or

within more traditional, print-based research materials?

I then ask students to apply the above criteria to each research

paper that I assign. Students come to realize that they need to

research more than one source of information and that they must

develop the ability to discern information and to filter bias and opin-ions

from the objective facts. In teaching and advocating this type

of methodology and critical thinking process, I also try to function

as a role model and demonstrate to students how to filter through

a plethora of information to find the sources that are best suited to

the task.

ALANNA KING

Orangeville District Secondary School, Orangeville, ON

One technique I use to help students critically evaluate research

obtained through internet sources is to show four examples of

websites and then walk the students through how to rank the sites

from worst to best. For this activity, I break the class into small groups

and ask students to develop their own criteria and to sort the exam-ples.

As a class, we then list all of the criteria together. Rather than

give the students the criteria in advance, they are expected to

actively engage with these exemplars for comparison and to express

their own ideas about the websites. Together, we develop a common

class lexicon for discussing the authority and reliability of websites

for research. Students now have a clear understanding of the expec-tations

associated with choosing internet research sources. Another

advantage of this activity is that the students become dissatisfied

with websites that are inadequate after interacting with good ones.

This activity also involves asking the students to engage with a

citation tool from the beginning of their research, which rein-forces

the value of finding quality internet sources. Most digital cita-tion

tools now produce excellent quality reference pages based on

user input. Poor websites have very little information to plug into

the citation tool, and sometimes students find that they cant even

locate an author or copyright date. Using the citation tool during

the research process, rather than at the end of an assignment, helps

students to manage their own research process.

Many students dont know how to use research material as evi-dence

for an argument, so finding deeper web-based sources is

critical to success. I think students undervalue their own voices and

dont know how to recognize and appreciate their own ideas during

research. I model how to analyze a paragraph on my digital projec-tor,

and think aloud in front of the students, so they can see how Im

developing ideas and connecting them to my research question.

Next, I give students a reading from a website and invite them to

listen to their own thoughts as they read silently. I suggest that these

thoughts might help to:

predict what is next

connect to their own experiences

connect to other texts they have experienced

connect to current events

During this close reading, students recognize once again the impor-tance

of choosing quality internet research. Engaging with the text

on multiple levels helps students learn how to critically evaluate web

sources.
34

Calculate your order
Pages (275 words)
Standard price: $0.00
Client Reviews
4.9
Sitejabber
4.6
Trustpilot
4.8
Our Guarantees
100% Confidentiality
Information about customers is confidential and never disclosed to third parties.
Original Writing
We complete all papers from scratch. You can get a plagiarism report.
Timely Delivery
No missed deadlines – 97% of assignments are completed in time.
Money Back
If you're confident that a writer didn't follow your order details, ask for a refund.

Calculate the price of your order

You will get a personal manager and a discount.
We'll send you the first draft for approval by at
Total price:
$0.00
Power up Your Academic Success with the
Team of Professionals. We’ve Got Your Back.
Power up Your Study Success with Experts We’ve Got Your Back.

Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code ESSAYHELP