bio stat

  
The insular cortex is thought to be

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

    

 involved in compassion and empathy, taste, perception, motor control,      self-awareness, cognitive functioning, and interpersonal experience. In      this paper, the focus is its role in substance abuse. Which of these is not mentioned by the authors in explaining      the link between the insular cortex and drug-seeking behaviors?
  

lesions in the   insular cortex interrupt drug-seeking behaviors

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

 

individuals with   substance abuse history showed reduced activity in the insular cortex 

 

there are no   drug-related receptors in the insular cortex

 

the anterior   agranular insula subregion contains dopamine and opioid receptors

10 points   

QUESTION 2

  1. The authors used 35 day      old rats because

  

35 days is   adolescent, when initial drug exposure occurs often in humans

 

35 days corresponds   to the age at which rats in the wild begin buying drugs

 

Rats do not develop   an insular cortex until 30 days

 

The rat brain has   no insular cortex so it doesn’t matter

10 points   

QUESTION 3

  1. How did limited and      extended  intravenous self-administration training differ?

  
10 points   

QUESTION 4

  1. Did pressing the lever      during a test result in a predictable delivery of cocaine or an      unpredictable dose? Why do you think it was done this way?

    
10 points   

QUESTION 5

  1. Did cocaine-seeking      behaviors increase or decrease on withdrawal day 45, relative to      withdrawal day 1? Did this effect depend on the amount or duration of      prior cocaine use (that is, limited or extended)?

    
10 points   

QUESTION 6

  1. An action potential      occurs when the membrane potential of a specific cell location rapidly      rises and falls; this is an essential part of neuron signaling.

         What is meant by  rise time, decay time, half‐amplitude duration ,      and amplitude of action potentials?

     

  

14px

P

0 WORDS

POWERED BY TINY

10 points   

QUESTION 7

  1. How are the rise time,      decay time, half‐amplitude duration, and amplitude of action potentials in      pyramidal neurons of the insular cortex affected by cocaine training?

    
10 points   

QUESTION 8

  1. When a depolarizing      current is injected into a nerve cell, the resultant decrease in the      membrane potential activates voltage-dependent Na+ channels. The      activation of the Na+ channels in turn accelerates the depolarization      process, producing the rising phase of the action potential.
         This causes the neuron to fire, or spike.

         Injecting depolarizing current is done to assess whether the sensitivity      of a neuron is changed by a procedure (which would be measurable as a      change in the spike rate of the neuron).

         In this paper, would you say that cocaine administration was shown to      alter the sensitivity of insula pyramidal neurons to stimulation? Why or      why not?

10 points   

QUESTION 9

  1. For any given neuron,      there is also a threshold of action potential. What does that mean?
  2. Is the action potential      threshold shown to be changed by limited or extended cocaine training? How      do you know this?

    
10 points   

QUESTION 11

  1. It is generally thought      that extended drug exposure has more robust effects on drug‐seeking and      drug‐taking behaviors than limited use. Does this study support or      contradict this bit of conventional wisdom? How?

10 points   

QUESTION 12

  1. In this model, rats      learn that pressing a lever is rewarded with an injection of cocaine.      However, some rats with a history of extended training seem not to press      the lever. What might explain this?

    
10 points   

QUESTION 13

  1. What is the main      difference (as the authors describe it) between the methods of this study      and those of other rat cocaine studies?

196  |  CNS Neurosci Ther. 2021;27:196–205.wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/cns

1   |   I N T R O D U C T I O N

Addiction is a mental disorder defined by compulsive and chronic drug

taking. The development of drug abuse after an extended period of drug
taking has been suggested to depend on a shift from early recreational
drug use to later compulsive drug abuse. The neurobiological mechanisms

leading to this shift remain unknown. Furthermore, compared with a lim-
ited drug history, extended drug exposure was assumed to have a higher
probability to lead to prolonged neuronal alterations, resulting in a higher
risk of drug relapse and a further extension of drug abuse.1-4

The insular cortex (IC), or insula for short, plays an important
role in drug addiction. Both human5-7 and animal studies8 showed

Received: 30 July 2020  |  Revised: 19 September 2020  |  Accepted: 23 September 2020
DOI: 10.1111/cns.13469

O R I G I N A L A R T I C L E

Limited versus extended cocaine intravenous self-
administration: Behavioral effects and electrophysiological
changes in insular cortex

Yi-Xiao Luo1,2 | Donald Huang1 | Changyong Guo1 | Yao-Ying Ma1,2,3

This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly cited.
© 2020 The Authors. CNS Neuroscience & Therapeutics published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd

Luo and Huang are contributed equally to this work.

1Department of Pharmacology and
Toxicology, Indiana University School of
Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
2Department of Psychology, Behavioral
Neuroscience Program, State University of
New York, Binghamton, NY, USA
3Stark Neurosciences Research Institute,
Indiana University School of Medicine,
Indianapolis, IN, USA

Correspondence
Yao-Ying Ma, Department of Pharmacology
and Toxicology, Indiana University School of
Medicine, 635 Barnhill Drive, Indianapolis,
IN 46202, USA.
Email: ym9@iu.edu

Funding information
Brain & Behavior Research Foundation
grant, Grant/Award Number: 24989;
National Institute on Alcohol Abuse
and Alcoholism, Grant/Award Number:
R01AA025784

Abstract
Aims: Limited vs extended drug exposure has been proposed as one of the key factors
in determining the risk of relapse, which is the primary characteristic of addiction
behaviors. The current studies were designed to explore the related behavioral
effects and neuronal alterations in the insular cortex (IC), an important brain region
involved in addiction.
Methods: Experiments started with rats at the age of 35 days, a typical adolescent
stage when initial drug exposure occurs often in humans. The drug-seeking/taking
behaviors, and membrane properties and intrinsic excitability of IC pyramidal neurons
were measured on withdrawal day (WD) 1 and WD 45-48 after limited vs extended
cocaine intravenous self-administration (IVSA).
Results: We found higher cocaine-taking behaviors at the late withdrawal period
after limited vs extended cocaine IVSA. We also found minor but significant effects
of limited but not extended cocaine exposure on the kinetics and amplitude of action
potentials on WD 45, in IC pyramidal neurons.
Conclusion: Our results indicate potential high risks of relapse in young rats with
limited but not extended drug exposure, although the adaptations detected in the IC
may not be sufficient to explain the neural changes of higher drug-taking behaviors
induced by limited cocaine IVSA.

K E Y W O R D S

cocaine, drug seeking, drug taking, whole-cell patch-clamp, insula cortex

www.wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/cns

mailto:

https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3735-8412

http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/

mailto:ym9@iu.edu

http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1111%2Fcns.13469&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2020-10-29

     |  197LUO et aL.

that IC lesions interrupt addictive behaviors, suggesting that the
IC or the IC-mediated interoceptive system is sensitized in ad-
dicted subjects as a consequence of addiction history and a cause
of relapse. However, a different picture was suggested by neuro-
imaging studies, in which reduced activity9,10 and shrunken gray
matter5,11 in the IC were detected in individuals with substance
abuse history. These apparently contradictory lines of evidence
may indicate that the IC can both promote drug use (eg, via in-
creased perception of craving for drugs) and weaken the processes
that prevent drug use (such as decision-making and the evaluation
of negative consequences).12 The IC consists of 3 subregions, that
is, posterior granular insular, anterior agranular insula (AAI), and
the intermediate dysgranular insula).13-15 Particular attention in
addiction research has been payed to the AAI due to its rich che-
moarchitecture, including the presence of dopamine and opioid
receptors.6,16-20

The present study used a rat model of cocaine intravenous
self-administration (IVSA) to explore the behavioral and electro-
physiological consequences of limited vs extended IVSA after short
(1 day) or prolonged (45 days) withdrawal periods (designated as
WD1 and WD45, respectively). Experiments started with rats at the
age of 35 days, a typical adolescent stage when initial drug exposure
occurs often in humans.21,22

2   |   M AT E R I A L S A N D M E T H O D S

2.1 | Experimental subjects

All procedures were performed in accordance with the United
States Public Health Service Guide for Care and Use of Laboratory
Animals and were approved by the institutional Animal Care and
Use committee at the State University of New York, Binghamton
and Indiana University School of Medicine. Experiments were
conducted on male Sprague Dawley rats, bred in-house using
breeders originally derived from Envigo (USA). With the day of
birth being deemed as postnatal day (P) 0, rats were allowed to
develop normally until P21-23 when animals were weaned and
pair-housed in standard Plexiglas bins unless receiving cannula
implantation procedure. Rats were maintained on a 7 am/7 pm light/
dark schedule with ad libitum access to food and water. Rats were
allowed 5-7 days to acclimate to colony conditions and handled to
habituate them to human contact prior to experimentation. Similar
to our previous studies, rats received catheter implantation on
postnatal ~35 days. Seven days later, rats received one overnight
session, followed by 5 of 1-hour daily sessions (considered
as limited training procedure) or 21 of 6-hour daily sessions
(considered as extended training procedure) of cocaine IVSA
training.23,24 In total, 75 rats were used in this study, among which
8 rats were excluded due to patency failure of the catheter (n = 6)
and behavioral outliers (n = 2). From the 67 remaining subjects,
21 were trained by limited (n = 11) or extended (n = 10) cocaine

IVSA procedures, followed by cocaine-seeking test on WD1 and
WD45, and cocaine-taking test on WD46-48 (Figure 1A). The
other 46 rats were trained by limited (n = 9 for saline and n = 14
for cocaine) or extended (n = 10 for saline and n = 13 for cocaine)
IVSA procedures and whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were
obtained either 1 or 45 days after the last IVSA session.

2.2 | Intravenous self-administration of cocaine

2.2.1 | Catheter implantation

As described previously,23-27 a silastic catheter was inserted into
the right jugular vein, and the distal end was led subcutaneously to
the back between the scapulae. Catheters were constructed from
silastic tubing (~8 cm; inner diameter 0.020 in, outer diameter 0.037
in) connected to a One Channel Rat Button (Instech Labs). Rats
were allowed to recover for ~7 days. During recovery, the catheter
was flushed daily with 1 mL/kg body weight of heparin (10 U/mL)
and gentamicin antibiotic (5 mg/mL) in sterile saline to help protect
against infection and catheter occlusion.

2.2.2 | Self-administration apparatus

Experiments were conducted in operant-conditioning chambers
enclosed within sound-attenuating cabinets (Med Associates).
Each chamber contained two levers randomly assigned as active vs
inactive levers, a food dispenser, and the conditioned stimulus (CS)
light 9 cm above each lever. No food or water was provided in the
chambers during the training or testing sessions.

2.2.3 | Intravenous cocaine self-
administration training

~7 days after catheter implantation, cocaine self-administration
training began with an overnight session at 7 pm to 7 am on the
following day. The daily training session, either 1 hour per day for
5 days as limited cocaine access or 6 hours per day for 21 days as
extended cocaine access, started on the day after. The same training
protocol was used in overnight and daily sessions. Rats were placed in
the self-administration chamber on a fixed ratio (FR) 1 reinforcement
schedule with the house light on. Active lever press resulted in a
cocaine infusion (0.75 mg/kg over 2-4 seconds) and illumination
of a CS light above the active lever for 20 seconds with the house
light off. In contrast, the inactive lever press led to no outcome but
was also recorded. Rats that received at least 60 cocaine rewards
in the overnight session were allowed to move to daily self-
administration of cocaine ~24 hr after the overnight training on an
FR1 reinforcement schedule. Animals that did not meet this standard
(n = 1) were removed from subsequent self-administration training.

198  |     LUO et aL.

2.2.4 | Cocaine-seeking test

Rats previously assigned for behavioral test were tested on both
WD1 and WD45 for their cocaine-seeking behaviors. The co-
caine-seeking test lasted for 1 hour, during which rats were re-
exposed to the operant chamber with the exact same conditions
as during cocaine IVSA training except no cocaine IV delivery was
programmed after active lever press. The active lever press during
1 hour seeking test was analyzed as an index of cocaine-seeking
level (Figure 1D).

2.2.5 | Cocaine-taking test

Multiple doses of cocaine were evaluated in the cocaine-taking test
as described before.28 Briefly, a 2-hour period was divided into four
30-minute segments, which allowed the assessment of four pseudo-
randomized doses of cocaine (0.1, 0.3, 1.0, and 3.0 mg/kg per in-
jection) in a 2-hour session. Two adjacent 30-minute sessions were
spaced by a ~5-minute interval. During each 30-minute session,
the exact same conditions, although the dose of cocaine per injec-
tion was different, were applied to rats, which means rats received

F I G U R E 1   Effects limited vs extended cocaine IVSA on cocaine-seeking and cocaine-taking behaviors during the withdrawal period. A,
Timeline of cocaine-seeking and cocaine-taking test with specified age of days at each stage. Rats were trained by limited (upper line) or
extended (lower line) cocaine IVSA procedures. P, postnatal days. B, The daily # of cocaine IV infusion and daily # of active lever press by
rats exposed to limited cocaine IVSA procedure. C, The daily # of cocaine IV infusion and daily # of active lever press by rats exposed to
extended cocaine IVSA procedure. D, Cocaine-taking behaviors on WD1 and WD45. E, Cocaine-taking behaviors tested by multiple dose
of cocaine (ie, 0.1, 0.3, 1.0, 3.0 mg/kg per infusion) on WD46-48. F, cocaine (0.1 mg/kg per infusion)-taking behaviors of individual rat are
represented by the circles in the columns. Data were analyzed by one-way ANOVA (each curve in B, C) and two-way ANOVA with repeated
measures (D,E), followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests or Student’s t test (F). n = 11 in limited cocaine IVSA group; n = 10 in extended
cocaine IVSA group. *P < .05; **P < .01, comparations between day 1 vs any of other days during the IVSA training (C), WD1 vs WD45 (D), or limited vs extended IVSA groups (E,F)

     |  199LUO et aL.

cocaine IV delivery after active lever press, followed by a 20-second
timeout during which the cue light signals were presented and no co-
caine delivery was allowed. This 2-hour daily session was done once
per day for 3 consecutive days (ie, WD46-48). The average number
of cocaine infusions from 3 30-minute sessions with a specific dose
of cocaine was analyzed as an index of cocaine-taking level.

2.3 | Brain slice whole-cell patch-clamp recordings

Standard procedures were used for preparing slices and
whole-cell patch-clamp recordings as detailed in our previous
publications.23,24,27 Before sacrifice, the rats were anesthetized
with isoflurane and subsequently transcardially perfused with 4°C
cutting solution (in mM: 135 N-methyl-D-glucamine, 1 KCl, 1.2
KH2PO4, 0.5 CaCl2, 1.5 MgCl2, 20 choline-HCO3, 11 glucose, pH
adjusted to 7.4 with HCl, and saturated with 95% O2/5% CO2).
The rat was decapitated, and then the brain was removed and
glued to a block before slicing using a Leica VT1200s vibratome
in 4°C cutting solution. Coronal slices of 250-µm thickness were
cut such that the preparation contained the signature anatomical
landmarks (eg, the rhinal fissure, the anterior commissure, and
the corpus callosum) that clearly delineate the A AI area. After
allowing at least 1 hour for recovery, slices were transferred from
a holding chamber to a submerged recording chamber where it
was continuously perfused with oxygenated ACSF maintained at
30 ± 1°C.

Standard whole-cell current- or voltage-clamp recordings were
obtained with a MultiClamp 700B amplifier (Molecular Devices), fil-
tered at 3 kHz, amplified 5 times, and then digitized at 20 kHz with
a Digidata 1550B analog-to-digital converter (Molecular Devices).
The recording electrodes (3-5 MΩ) were filled with (in mmol/L): 108
KMeSO3, 20 KCl, 0.4 K-EGTA, 10 HEPES, 2.5 Mg-ATP, 0.25 Na-GTP,
7.5 phosphocreatine (Na2), 1 L-glutathione, 2 MgCl2, pH 7.3. The
recording bath solution contained (in mM): 119 NaCl, 2.5 KCl, 2.5
CaCl2, 1.3 MgCl2, 1 NaH2PO4, 26.2 NaHCO3, and 11 glucose, satu-
rated with 95% O2/ 5% CO2 at 30 ± 1°C. Details for whole-cell patch-
clamp recordings can be found in one of our previous publications.27
Cells were patched in voltage clamp mode and held at −70 mV. Cell
membrane capacitance (Cm), input resistance (Rm), and time con-
stant (τ) were calculated by applying a depolarizing step voltage
command (5 mV) and using the membrane test function integrated in
the pClamp10 software. Then, recordings were switched to current
clamp mode. Resting membrane potential was measured and then
adjusted to −70 mV through injection of positive current (50-100 pA).
The intrinsic excitability was examined using a series of depolarizing
current pulses and by constructing input-output (I-O) functions.

2.4 | Data acquisition and analysis

Data were collected either 2 days or 21 days after the last IVSA
session. All results are shown as mean ± SEM. Each experiment
was replicated in at least 10 rats for in vivo behavioral tests and at

F I G U R E 2   Effects of limited vs extended cocaine IVSA on the kinetics of action potentials. A, A sample trace showing the second
action potential induced by 300 pA injection current was analyzed for the spike kinetics. B, A sample trace of action potential showing the
measurement of rise time, decay time, and amplitude. C, Sample trace of action potential from the pyramidal neurons in the IC 1 d (left)
and 45 d (right) after limited saline (denoted “S”)/ cocaine (denoted “C”) IVSA training. D, E, Summarized data showing that, relative to the
saline controls, limited cocaine treatment significantly prolonged the rise time (D), decay time (E), and half-amplitude duration (F) of action
potential 45 d but not 1 d after IVSA training. G, Summarized data showing that limited cocaine treatment significantly decreased the
amplitude of action potential on WD45, relative to WD1. H, Sample trace of action potential from the pyramidal neurons in the IC 1 d (left)
and 45 d (right) after extended saline (in gray)/ cocaine (in black) IVSA training. I-L, Summarized data showing no difference between saline
controls vs extended cocaine-treated rats in rise time (I), decay time (J), half-amplitude duration (K), or amplitude (L) of action potential on
both 1 d and 45 d after IVSA training. Data were analyzed by two-way ANOVA, followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests. n/m, the number of
cells/animals for data collection. *P < .05

200  |     LUO et aL.

least 4 rats (usually 2-4 cells per animal were recorded per group)
for in vitro electrophysiological recordings. Sample size is pre-
sented as n, referring to the number of rats in behavioral tests, or
presented as m/n, where “m” refers to the number of cells exam-
ined and “n” refers to the number of rats used for electrophysi-
ology experiments. Statistical significance was assessed using
the Student t test (Figure 1F), one-way ANOVA with repeated
measures (each individual curve in Figure 1B,C), two-way ANOVA
(Figures 2D-G,I-L and 4A-D), or two-way ANOVA with repeated
measures (Figures 1D,E and 3B,D,F,H), followed by Bonferroni
post-hoc tests. The analytical unit is the animal in behavioral data
(Figure 1), or both the cell and the animal in the in vitro slice record-
ing data (Figure 2-4).

3   |   R E S U LT S

3.1 | Effects of limited vs extended cocaine IVSA on
cocaine-seeking and cocaine-taking behaviors during
the withdrawal period

Adolescent rats (n = 21), implanted with catheter on postnatal day
35 and recovered for ~1 week, were trained by limited (ie, 1 hour

per day for 5 days) or extended (ie, 6 hours per day for 21 days)
cocaine IVSA procedure, followed by a period of forced abstinence
during which the rats were just housed in the home cage, except on
WD1 and WD45 when they underwent the cocaine-seeking test,
and on WD46-48 when they received the cocaine-taking test in
the operant chambers as shown in Figure 1A. A flat curve of the
daily cocaine infusion (F4,40 = 1.4, P = .24) and the number of active
lever presses (F4,40 = 2.2, P = .09) was observed during the 5 day
of limited cocaine IVSA training sessions (Figure 1B). An increas-
ing number of daily cocaine infusions (F20,180 = 24.9, P < .01) and active lever presses (F20,180 = 8.0, P < .01) were observed during 21-day extended cocaine IVSA sessions (Figure 1C). During the withdrawal period, the cocaine-seeking behaviors were increased on WD45, compared with that on WD1, regardless of the limited vs extended cocaine history (limited/extended F1,18 = 0.2, P = .20; WD1/WD45 F1,18 = 62.4, P < .01; limited/extended × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,18 = 2.0, P = .17; Figure 1D). Interestingly, rats with a history of limited cocaine IVSA showed an upward shift of dose- response curve (ie, the dose of cocaine and the cocaine-taking response), compared with that from the extended group (limited/ extended × cocaine dose interaction F3,57 = 3.0, P = .04; limited/ extended F1,19 = 7.9, P = .01; cocaine dose F3,57 = 60.4, P < .01; Figure 1E), partially demonstrated at the dose of 0.1 mg/kg cocaine

F I G U R E 3   Effects of limited vs extended cocaine IVSA on spike number of pyramidal neurons in the IC induced by current injections.
A, B, Example traces (A) and summarized data (B) showing no significant effects of limited cocaine IVSA on spike number 1 d after IVSA
training. C, D, Example traces (C) and summarized data (D) showing no significant effects of limited cocaine IVSA on spike number 45 d after
IVSA training. E, F, Example traces (E) and summarized data (F) showing no significant effects of extended cocaine IVSA on spike number
1 d after IVSA training. G, H, Example traces (G) and summarized data (H) showing no significant effects of extended cocaine IVSA on spike
number 45 d after IVSA training. Data were analyzed by two-way ANOVA with repeated measures, followed by Bonferroni post-hoc tests.
n/m, the number of cells/animals for data collection

     |  201LUO et aL.

per infusion (t19 = 2.5, P = .02; Figure 1F). Drug-seeking and drug-
taking behaviors after a prolonged withdrawal period have been
well accepted as reliable indices of relapse.29-31 Thus, we conclude
that the extended cocaine IVSA procedure used in the current
study did not increase the risk for relapse. Interestingly, the limited
cocaine IVSA procedure may increase the relapse risk due to sig-
nificantly increased drug-taking behaviors on WD45.

3.2 | Effects of limited vs extended cocaine IVSA
on the kinetics and amplitude of evoked action
potentials in insula pyramidal neurons

Whole-cell current clamp recordings in IC-containing coronal
slices were performed in rats 1 day or 45 days after IVSA of saline
or cocaine. The kinetics and amplitude of the action potential were
analyzed by measuring the 2nd action potential induced by 300 pA
current injection (Figure 2A). The insula pyramidal neurons from
rats with a history of limited IVSA procedure, that is, 1 hour per day
for 5 days, displayed significantly increased rise time (Figure 2D,
saline/cocaine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,65 = 4.1, P = .04, sa-
line/cocaine F1,65 = 21.3, P < .01, and WD1/WD45 F1,65 = 0.1, P = .80, cell-based; saline/cocaine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,19 = 4.4, P = .049, saline/cocaine F1,19 = 12.1, P < .01, and WD1/ WD45 F1,19 = 0.1, P = .66, animal-based), decay time (Figure 2E,

saline/cocaine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,65 = 4.2, P = .04, sa-
line/cocaine F1,65 = 30.0, P < .01, and WD1/WD45 F1,65 = 4.9, P = .03, cell-based; saline/cocaine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,19 = 4.5, P = .047, saline/cocaine F1,19 = 27.3, P < .01, and WD1/ WD45 F1,19 = 4.5, P = .047, animal-based), half-amplitude duration (Figure 2F, saline/cocaine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,65 = 4.7, P = .03, saline/cocaine F1,65 = 36.4, P < .01, and WD1/WD45 F1,65 = 3.0, P = .08, cell-based; saline/cocaine × WD1/WD45 in- teraction F1,19 = 4.6, P = .045, saline/cocaine F1,19 = 24.9, P < .01, and WD1/WD45 F1,19 = 2.7, P = .12, animal-based), and decreased amplitude of the evoked action potential (Figure 2G, saline/co- caine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,65 = 14.1, P < .01, saline/co- caine F1,65 = 6.9, P = .01, and WD1/WD45 F1,65 = 9.0, P < .01, cell-based; saline/cocaine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,19 = 10.8, P = .01, saline/cocaine F1,19 = 6.4, P = .02, and WD1/WD45 F1,19 = 8.4, P = 0.01, animal-based) 45 days, but not 1 day, after cocaine IVSA, compared with saline controls. However, no sig- nificant differences in rise time (Figure 2I, saline/cocaine × WD1/ WD45 interaction F1,68 = 0.23, P = .63, saline/cocaine F1,68 = 0.5, P = .50, and WD1/WD45 F1,68 = 1.6, P = .22, cell-based; saline/ cocaine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,19 = 0.4, P = .53, saline/co- caine F1,19 = 1.3, P = .27, and WD1/WD45 F1,19 = 0.5, P = .49, animal-based), decay time (Figure 2J, saline/cocaine × WD1/ WD45 interaction F1,68 = 0.2, P = .67, saline/cocaine F1,68 = 1.0, P = .32, and WD1/WD45 F1,68 = 1.4, P = .24, cell-based; saline/

F I G U R E 4   Effects of limited vs
extended cocaine IVSA on RMP and
action potential threshold. A, Summarized
data showing no significant effects of
limited IVSA on RMP in insula pyramidal
neurons. B, Summarized data showing
no significant effects of limited IVSA
on action potential threshold in insula
pyramidal neurons. C, Summarized data
showing no significant effects of extended
IVSA on RMP in insula pyramidal
neurons. D, Summarized data showing
no significant effects of limited IVSA
on action potential threshold in insula
pyramidal neurons. Data were analyzed by
two-way ANOVA, followed by Bonferroni
post-hoc tests. n/m, the number of cells/
animals for data collection

202  |     LUO et aL.

cocaine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,19 = 0.4, P = .54, saline/co-
caine F1,19 = 0.8, P = .37, and WD1/WD45 F1,19 = 1.5, P = .25,
animal-based), half-amplitude duration (Figure 2K, saline/co-
caine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,68 = 0.1, P = .75, saline/co-
caine F1,68 = 1.2, P = .28, and WD1/WD45 F1,68 = 0.4, P = .51,
cell-based; saline/cocaine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,19 = 0.3,
P = .57, saline/cocaine F1,19 = 1.1, P = .30, and WD1/WD45
F1,19 = 0.6, P = .44, animal-based), and action potential amplitude
(Figure 2L, saline/cocaine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,68 = 2.1,
P = .15, saline/cocaine F1,68 < 0.1, P = .88, and WD1/WD45 F1,68 = 0.5, P = .49, cell-based; saline/cocaine × WD1/WD45 in- teraction F1,19 = 2.3, P = .15, saline/cocaine F1,19 = 0.1, P = .42, and WD1/WD45 F1,19 = 0.4, P = .55, animal-based) were detected in rats with an extended IVSA (ie, 6 hr per day for 21 days) of sa- line vs cocaine either 1 day or 45 days after the last IVSA training session, demonstrating significant kinetic changes of APs after a prolonged withdrawal period from limited but not extended IVSA of cocaine.

3.3 | Effects of limited vs extended cocaine IVSA on
intrinsic excitability of insula pyramidal neurons

The number of spikes evoked by depolarizing current injections
ranging from 50 to 400 pA were also analyzed. Our data showed
that, relative to saline controls, cocaine IVSA procedure did not
affect the number of action potentials evoked 1 day vs 45 days
after the last IVSA session (Figure 3). Specifically, neither
significant effects of limited cocaine IVSA on spike number were
detected 1 day after IVSA training (Figure 3B, saline/cocaine × Iinj
interaction F7,266 = 0.5, P = .85, and saline/cocaine F1,38 < 0.1, P = .82, cell-based; saline/cocaine × Iinj interaction F7,77 = 0.5, P = .83, and saline/cocaine F1,11 < 0.1, P = .89, animal-based) or 45 days after IVSA training (Figure 3D, saline/cocaine × Iinj interaction F7,224 = 0.2, P = .98, and saline/cocaine F1,32 = 0.1, P = .77, cell-based; saline/cocaine × Iinj interaction F7,56 = 0.3, P = .95, and saline/cocaine F1,8 = 0.1, P = .77, animal-based), nor significant effects of extended cocaine IVSA on spike number were detected 1 day after IVSA training (Figure 3F, saline/cocaine × Iinj interaction F7,273 = 0.8, P = .60, and saline/cocaine F1,39 = 0.4, P = .55, cell-based; saline/cocaine × Iinj interaction F7,70 = 1.0, P = .46, and saline/cocaine F1,10 = 0.4, P = .54, animal-based) or 45 days after IVSA training (Figure 3H, saline/cocaine × Iinj interaction F7,266 = 0.2, P = .98, and saline/cocaine F1,38 = 0.1, P = .79, cell-based; saline/cocaine × Iinj interaction F7,63 = 0.2, P = .97, and saline/cocaine F1,9 = 0.1, P = .80, animal-based).

Further data analyses showed no significant difference of
RMPs in rats with a history with limited (Figure 4A, saline/co-
caine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,70 = 2.2, P = .14, saline/cocaine
F1,70 = 1.1, P = .30, and WD1/WD45 F1,70 = 0.1, P = .79, cell-
based; saline/cocaine × Iinj interaction F1,19 = 2.4, P = .14, saline/
cocaine × F1,19 = 0.9, P = .35, and WD1/WD45 F1,19 = 0.1, P = .80,
animal-based) or extended (Figure 4C, saline/cocaine × WD1/

WD45 interaction F1,77 = 3.3, P = .08, saline/cocaine F1,77 = 3.1,
P = .08, and WD1/WD45 F1,77 = 1.0, P = .34, cell-based; sa-
line/cocaine × Iinj interaction F1,19 = 3.0, P = .10, saline/co-
caine × F1,19 = 3.1, P = .09, and WD1/WD45 F1,19 = 1.0, P = .38,
animal-based) cocaine vs saline IVSA. Finally, limited (Figure 4B,
saline/cocaine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,70 = 0.1, P = .79,
saline/cocaine F1,70 < 0.1, P = .97, and WD1/WD45 F1,70 = 0.2, P = .67, cell-based; saline/cocaine × Iinj interaction F1,19 = 0.1, P = .78, saline/cocaine × F1,19 < 0.1, P = .95, and WD1/WD45 F1,19 = 0.1, P = .71, animal-based) or extended (Figure 4D, saline/ cocaine × WD1/WD45 interaction F1,77 = 0.3, P = .58, saline/co- caine F1,77 = 3.0, P = .09, and WD1/WD45 F1,77 = 1.6, P = .21, cell-based; saline/cocaine × Iinj interaction F1,19 = 0.4, P = .54, saline/cocaine × F1,19 = 2.7, P = .11, and WD1/WD45 F1,19 = 1.6, P = .22, animal-based) cocaine IVSA did not significantly influence the threshold of action potential. Thus, the membrane excitability of AAI pyramidal neurons was not affected by either limited or extended cocaine IVSA history.

4   |   D I S C U S S I O N

Our data demonstrated no differences of drug-seeking behaviors
after limited vs extended cocaine IVSA, although both limited and
extended IVSA rats showed higher drug-seeking behaviors on
WD45, compared with WD1, which indicates the duration of with-
drawal period can significantly increase the drug-seeking behav-
iors as evidenced before.32-35 Importantly, limited cocaine IVSA
significantly increased cocaine-taking behaviors compared with
the extended cocaine IVSA group as supported by the upward
shift of the dose-response curve of cocaine taking at the later
withdrawal stage. Thus, limited, compared with extended, cocaine
history, may lead to a potential high risk of relapse to cocaine.
Notably, this behavioral outcome dispels the current belief that
extended drug exposure has more robust effects on drug-seeking
and drug-taking behaviors. For example, extended access to drugs
significantly increased drug-taking behaviors in the withdrawal
period36-38 and compulsive drug-seeking behaviors could only
be established in rats by extended but not limited cocaine IVSA
procedure39. It is critical to point out the developmental stages
when drug exposure and drug withdrawal occur. Relative to adults,
adolescent rats took cocaine more readily, were more sensitive to
lower doses, and showed greater escalation of cocaine intake.40
Thus, our IVSA training was done at the adolescence stage. On
another hand, it has been fashionable in addiction research to run
drug exposure with extended daily sessions (≥6 hours) during a
long period (eg, at least 10 days).41-44 However, studies on limited
vs extended drug exposure in adolescent animals were still lacking.
Most, if not all, of the previous studies comparing the prolonged
effects of limited vs extended drug exposure on drug-seeking/tak-
ing behaviors used adult animals, whereas rats were adolescent
when the current experiments started. We found high risk of re-
lapse in rats with limited but not extended drugs at the adolescent

     |  203LUO et aL.

stage. Thus, it is assumed the developmental stage of drug experi-
ence matters more than the amount of drug in the young brain.

This is based on several observations: First, at the initial
drug-taking stage, young brains may be more sensitive to the
effects of drugs. This vulnerability of adolescents may be fully
masked by chronic drug administration.21,22 According to clinical
observations, the onset of drug use during adolescence appears
to lead to a prolonged sensitivity to drug relapse and dependence
in humans.45,46 Second, withdrawal experience of the young brain
may trigger more intensive neuronal alterations that facilitate the
drug-seeking and drug-taking behaviors even after a prolonged
withdrawal period. Our previous works demonstrated active neu-
ronal adaptations by the passage of the withdrawal period.23,24,47
The current limited cocaine IVSA procedure involves at least two
factors, which significantly increase the risk of drug-taking behav-
iors at the late withdrawal time. One is the limited exposure of
cocaine, which is enough for young brains to be reshaped with
the abusive potential of drugs. The other is the withdrawal period
starting from the adolescent stage when the brain is highly mallea-
ble. Our current conclusion, that is, high risk of relapse associated
with limited drug exposure at the adolescent stage, followed by a
withdrawal period, is not contradictory but an essential comple-
ment to the established addiction theory.

Low drug taking observed in rats with a history of extended co-
caine IVSA might be attributed to their poor learning and memory,
which resulted in a poor retrieval of the previously learned association
between the action (ie, lever press) and output (cocaine IV delivery and
cue light) in the seeking/taking test during the withdrawal period. This
can be excluded by the comparable drug-seeking behaviors between
rats pre-treated by limited vs extended cocaine IVSA on either WD1
or WD45, and comparable drug-taking behaviors at 10 mg/kg cocaine
per injection on WD46-48, indicating the retrieval of previous acquired
operant behaviors was intact. Also, the poor locomotion could be a
consequence of extended cocaine IVSA, which can be excluded by the
comparable number of inactive lever presses in rats treated by limited
vs extended cocaine IVSA (eg, active lever presses in 1-hour drug-seek-
ing test on WD45: limited, 10.1 ± 3.4; extended, 8.7 ± 2.7, P = .34).

Our data showed escalated drug intake in rats with extended
IVSA procedure, consistent with previous observations.48 However,
there are some exceptions; data from rhesus monkeys showed no
escalated intake over time after extended access to cocaine IVSA.49
We found that rats with escalated drug-taking behaviors during the
extended cocaine IVSA procedure did not show high risk of relapse.
Together with the previous report that the presence of escalation
did not affect reinstatement,48 we assume that the presence of esca-
lation has limited predictability on the risks of relapse. Escalation of
cocaine taking was produced by continuous cocaine IVSA.50

Although the critical involvement of the IC in addiction-related
behaviors has been widely accepted, little was known about the in-
trinsic excitability of IC pyramidal neurons in rats which experienced
limited vs extended IVSA. We found some minor but significant ef-
fects of limited but not extended cocaine exposure on the kinetics
and amplitude of action potentials on WD 45 after limited cocaine

IVSA, in pyramidal neurons from the AAI. Although our original hy-
pothesis was that, compared with the saline controls, extended, but
not limited, cocaine exposure may lead to significant changes (eg,
modified intrinsic excitability) in pyramidal neurons in the AAI, we
did not see any effects of extended cocaine IVSA on this electro-
physiological readout in insular pyramidal neurons. These findings
indicate that limited cocaine exposure has some, probably priming,
effects on the capacity of IC neurons to generate APs and these ef-
fects are compensated after extended cocaine exposure. It is worth
to note that the slower kinetics and smaller amplitude of action po-
tentials observed on WD45 after limited cocaine IVSA could be also
observed in unhealthy cells. Our cells in different groups were sim-
ilar in terms of access resistance and holding current evaluated by
Membrane Test in voltage clamp, indicating comparable quality of
cells among groups. Thus, the observed changes in kinetics and am-
plitude of action potentials might be attributable to the adaptations
of ion channels (eg, Na+ and K+).

The involvement of the IC in cocaine-related effects is well
supported by previous literature. First, molecular alterations in
the IC were detected 21 days but not 1 day after 5-day cocaine
exposure. Specifically, the cocaine challenge produced a decrease
in zif 268 expression after the 21-day, but not 1-day, withdrawal
period.51 Second, by using an extended procedure of cocaine ac-
cess, the anterior IC was found to mediate the maintenance of es-
calated cocaine-taking behaviors but not the initial development of
cocaine-taking behaviors.52 Third, different subregions of IC may
be involved in cocaine-seeking behaviors triggered by different
factors. Agranular IC was involved in drug context-induced rein-
statement of cocaine-seeking behavior without altering locomotor
activity.53 Anterior dorsal agranular IC participated in cue-associ-
ated cocaine-seeking behaviors,54 whereas rostral agranular IC was
reported to be related to cocaine-seeking behaviors intensified by
negative affect.55 Last but not least, human studies also demon-
strated changes in the IC, as it was reported by human imaging
studies that cocaine dependence is related to altered functional in-
teractions of the IC with prefrontal networks.56

5   |   C O N C L U S I O N S

Compared with the extended cocaine IVSA, adolescent limited
cocaine IVSA increased drug-taking behaviors, indicating the im-
portance of the developmental stage of initial drug exposure and
the withdrawal period. The minor neuronal adaptations in the AAI
may not be the primary neuronal substrate involved in high risk of
relapse induced by adolescent brief exposure to cocaine. Future
studies may refine the neuronal and molecular substrate explora-
tions in the IC by investigating other IC subregions, other types
of neurons such as GABAergic interneurons, changes in synaptic
transmission, etc. Obviously, other brain regions, such as nucleus
accumbens and mPFC, may be explored to identify the key neuro-
logical events which cause the limited drug exposure-induced high
drug-taking behaviors.

204  |     LUO et aL.

A C K N O W L E D G M E N T S
This work was supported by NIH grants (R01AA025784) and Brain &
Behavior Research Foundation grant #24989.

C O N F L I C T O F I N T E R E S T
The authors report no conflicts of interest.

D ATA AVA I L A B I L I T Y S TAT E M E N T
The authors confirm that the data supporting the findings of
this study are available (1) within this article and (2) from the
corresponding author upon reasonable request.

O R C I D
Yao-Ying Ma https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3735-8412

R E F E R E N C E S
1. Ben-Shahar OM, Szumlinski KK, Lominac KD, et al. Extended ac-

cess to cocaine self-administration results in reduced gluta-
mate function within the medial prefrontal cortex. Addict Biol.
2012;17(4):746-757.

2. Cohen A, Soleiman MT, Talia R, Koob GF, George O, Mandyam
CD. Extended access nicotine self-administration with periodic
deprivation increases immature neurons in the hippocampus.
Psychopharmacology. 2015;232(2):453-463.

3. Cozannet RL, Markou A, Kuczenski R. Extended-access, but not
limited-access, methamphetamine self-administration induces be-
havioral and nucleus accumbens dopamine response changes in
rats. Eur J Neurosci. 2013;38(10):3487-3495.

4. Lenoir M, Cantin L, Vanhille N, Serre F, Ahmed SH. Extended heroin
access increases heroin choices over a potent nondrug alternative.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38(7):1209-1220.

5. Franklin TR, Acton PD, Maldjian JA, et al. Decreased gray matter
concentration in the insular, orbitofrontal, cingulate, and temporal
cortices of cocaine patients. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51(2):134-142.

6. Naqvi NH, Rudrauf D, Damasio H, Bechara A. Damage to
the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking. Science.
2007;315(5811):531-534.

7. Damasio AR, Grabowski TJ, Bechara A, et al. Subcortical and corti-
cal brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nat
Neurosci. 2000;3(10):1049-1056.

8. Contreras M, Ceric F, Torrealba F. Inactivation of the interoceptive
insula disrupts drug craving and malaise induced by lithium. Science.
2007;318(5850):655-658.

9. Stewart JL, Connolly CG, May AC, Tapert SF, Wittmann M, Paulus
MP. Striatum and insula dysfunction during reinforcement learning
differentiates abstinent and relapsed methamphetamine-depen-
dent individuals. Addiction. 2014;109(3):460-471.

10. Stewart JL, May AC, Poppa T, Davenport PW, Tapert SF, Paulus MP.
You are the danger: attenuated insula response in methamphet-
amine users during aversive interoceptive decision-making. Drug
Alcohol Depend. 2014;142:110-119.

11. Mackey S, Paulus M. Are there volumetric brain differences asso-
ciated with the use of cocaine and amphetamine-type stimulants?
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(3):300-316.

12. Gogolla N. The insular cortex. Curr Biol. 2017;27(12):R580-R586.
13. Teofilovski G. Anatomic characteristics of the grivet monkey insula.

Acta Med Iugosl. 1984;38(4):241-246.
14. Allen GV, Saper CB, Hurley KM, Cechetto DF. Organization of vis-

ceral and limbic connections in the insular cortex of the rat. J Comp
Neurol. 1991;311(1):1-16.

15. Uddin LQ, Nomi JS, Hebert-Seropian B, Ghaziri J, Boucher O.
Structure and function of the human insula. J Clin Neurophysiol.
2017;34(4):300-306.

16. Gaspar P, Berger B, Febvret A, Vigny A, Henry JP. Catecholamine
innervation of the human cerebral cortex as revealed by compar-
ative immunohistochemistry of tyrosine hydroxylase and dopa-
mine-beta-hydroxylase. J Comp Neurol. 1989;279(2):249-271.

17. Suhara T, Yasuno F, Sudo Y, et al. Dopamine D2 receptors in the insu-
lar cortex and the personality trait of novelty seeking. Neuroimage.
2001;13(5):891-895.

18. Burkey AR, Carstens E, Wenniger JJ, Tang J, Jasmin L. An opioider-
gic cortical antinociception triggering site in the agranular insular
cortex of the rat that contributes to morphine antinociception. J
Neurosci. 1996;16(20):6612-6623.

19. Baumgartner U, Buchholz HG, Bellosevich A, et al. High opi-
ate receptor binding potential in the human lateral pain system.
Neuroimage. 2006;30(3):692-699.

20. Shillinglaw JE, Morrisett RA, Mangieri RA. Ethanol modulates gluta-
matergic transmission and NMDAR-mediated synaptic plasticity in
the agranular insular cortex. Front Pharmacol. 2018;9:1458.

21. Li C, Frantz KJ. Attenuated incubation of cocaine seeking in male
rats trained to self-administer cocaine during periadolescence.
Psychopharmacology. 2009;204(4):725-733.

22. Doherty JM, Frantz KJ. Heroin self-administration and rein-
statement of heroin-seeking in adolescent vs. adult male rats.
Psychopharmacology. 2012;219(3):763-773.

23. Ma Y Y, Lee BR, Wang X, et al. Bidirectional modulation of incu-
bation of cocaine craving by silent synapse-based remodel-
ing of prefrontal cortex to accumbens projections. Neuron.
2014;83(6):1453-1467.

24. Ma YY, Wang X, Huang Y, et al. Re-silencing of silent synapses un-
masks anti-relapse effects of environmental enrichment. Proc Nat
Acad Sci USA. 2016;113(18):5089–5094.

25. Mu P, Moyer JT, Ishikawa M, et al. Exposure to cocaine dynamically
regulates the intrinsic membrane excitability of nucleus accumbens
neurons. J Neurosci. 2010;30(10):3689-3699.

26. Brown TE, Lee BR, Mu P, et al. A silent synapse-based mecha-
nism for cocaine-induced locomotor sensitization. J Neurosci.
2011;31(22):8163-8174.

27. Ma YY, Cepeda C, Chatta P, Franklin L, Evans CJ, Levine MS.
Regional and cell-type-specific effects of DAMGO on striatal D1
and D2 dopamine receptor-expressing medium-sized spiny neu-
rons. ASN Neuro. 2012;4(2):AN20110063.

28. Gancarz AM, Wang ZJ, Schroeder GL, et al. Activin receptor sig-
naling regulates cocaine-primed behavioral and morphological plas-
ticity. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(7):959-961.

29. Weiss F. Advances in animal models of relapse for addiction re-
search. In: Kuhn CM, Koob GF, eds. Advances in the Neuroscience of
Addiction. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press;2010.

30. Marchant NJ, Li X, Shaham Y. Recent developments in animal mod-
els of drug relapse. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2013;23(4):675-683.

31. Li TK. Clinical perspectives for the study of craving and relapse in
animal models. Addiction. 2000;95(Suppl 2):S55-S60.

32. Gancarz-Kausch AM, Adank DN, Dietz DM. Prolonged withdrawal
following cocaine self-administration increases resistance to pun-
ishment in a cocaine binge. Sci Rep. 2014;4:6876.

33. Lu L, Grimm JW, Hope BT, Shaham Y. Incubation of cocaine craving
after withdrawal: a review of preclinical data. Neuropharmacology.
2004;47(Suppl 1):214-226.

34. Grimm JW, Lu L, Hayashi T, Hope BT, Su TP, Shaham Y. Time-
dependent increases in brain-derived neurotrophic factor protein
levels within the mesolimbic dopamine system after withdrawal
from cocaine: implications for incubation of cocaine craving. J
Neurosci. 2003;23(3):742-747.

https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3735-8412

https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3735-8412

     |  205LUO et aL.

35. Grimm JW, Hope BT, Wise RA, Neuroadaptation SY. Incubation of
cocaine craving after withdrawal. Nature. 2001;412(6843):141-142.

36. Ahmed SH, Kenny PJ, Koob GF, Markou A. Neurobiological evi-
dence for hedonic allostasis associated with escalating cocaine use.
Nat Neurosci. 2002;5(7):625-626.

37. Ahmed SH, Walker JR, Koob GF. Persistent increase in the mo-
tivation to take heroin in rats with a history of drug escalation.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22(4):413-421.

38. Edwards S, Koob GF. Escalation of drug self-administration as
a hallmark of persistent addiction liability. Behav Pharmacol.
2013;24(5-6):356-362.

39. Vanderschuren LJ, Everitt BJ. Drug seeking becomes com-
pulsive after prolonged cocaine self-administration. Science.
2004;305(5686):1017-1019.

40. Wong WC, Ford KA, Pagels NE, McCutcheon JE, Marinelli
M. Adolescents are more vulnerable to cocaine addiction:
behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. J Neurosci.
2013;33(11):4913-4922.

41. Bossert JM, Hoots JK, Fredriksson I, et al. Role of mu, but not delta
or kappa, opioid receptors in context-induced reinstatement of
oxycodone seeking. Eur J Neurosci. 2019;50(3):2075-2085.

42. Neelakantan H, Holliday ED, Fox RG, et al. Lorcaserin suppresses
oxycodone self-administration and relapse vulnerability in rats. ACS
Chem Neurosci. 2017;8(5):1065-1073.

43. Wade CL, Vendruscolo LF, Schlosburg JE, Hernandez DO, Koob GF.
Compulsive-like responding for opioid analgesics in rats with ex-
tended access. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(2):421-428.

44. Zorrilla EP, Wee S, Zhao Y, et al. Extended access cocaine self-ad-
ministration differentially activates dorsal raphe and amyg-
dala corticotropin-releasing factor systems in rats. Addict Biol.
2012;17(2):300-308.

45. Laviola G, Adriani W, Terranova ML, Gerra G. Psychobiological risk
factors for vulnerability to psychostimulants in human adolescents
and animal models. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1999;23(7):993-1010.

46. O’Malley PM, Johnston LD. Drugs and driving by American high school
seniors, 2001–2006. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2007;68(6):834-842.

47. Lee BR, Ma YY, Huang YH, et al. Maturation of silent synapses in
amygdala-accumbens projection contributes to incubation of co-
caine craving. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16(11):1644-1651.

48. Knackstedt LA, Kalivas PW. Extended access to cocaine self-admin-
istration enhances drug-primed reinstatement but not behavioral
sensitization. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2007;322(3):1103-1109.

49. Kirkland Henry P, Davis M, Howell LL. Effects of cocaine self-ad-
ministration history under limited and extended access conditions
on in vivo striatal dopamine neurochemistry and acoustic startle in
rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology. 2009;205(2):237-247.

50. Calipari ES, Ferris MJ, Jones SR. Extended access of cocaine
self-administration results in tolerance to the dopamine-elevat-
ing and locomotor-stimulating effects of cocaine. J Neurochem.
2014;128(2):224-232.

51. Unal CT, Beverley JA, Willuhn I, Steiner H. Long-lasting dysregu-
lation of gene expression in corticostriatal circuits after repeated
cocaine treatment in adult rats: effects on zif 268 and homer 1a. Eur
J Neurosci. 2009;29(8):1615-1626.

52. Rotge JY, Cocker PJ, Daniel ML, Belin-Rauscent A, Everitt BJ, Belin
D. Bidirectional regulation over the development and expres-
sion of loss of control over cocaine intake by the anterior insula.
Psychopharmacology. 2017;234(9-10):1623-1631.

53. Arguello AA, Wang R, Lyons CM, Higginbotham JA, Hodges MA,
Fuchs RA. Role of the agranular insular cortex in contextual con-
trol over cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology.
2017;234(16):2431-2441.

54. Cosme CV, Gutman AL, LaLumiere RT. The dorsal agranular insu-
lar cortex regulates the cued reinstatement of cocaine-seeking,
but not food-seeking, Behavior in Rats. Neuropsychopharmacology.
2015;40(10):2425-2433.

55. Moschak TM, Wang X, Carelli RM. A neuronal ensemble in
the rostral agranular insula tracks cocaine-induced devalua-
tion of natural reward and predicts cocaine seeking. J Neurosci.
2018;38(39):8463–8472.

56. Cisler JM, Elton A, Kennedy AP, et al. Altered functional connec-
tivity of the insular cortex across prefrontal networks in cocaine
addiction. Psychiatry Res. 2013;213(1):39-46.

How to cite this article: Luo Y-X, Huang D, Guo C, Ma Y-Y.
Limited versus extended cocaine intravenous self-
administration: Behavioral effects and electrophysiological
changes in insular cortex. CNS Neurosci Ther. 2021;27:196–
205. https://doi.org/10.1111/cns.13469

https://doi.org/10.1111/cns.13469

Calculate your order
Pages (275 words)
Standard price: $0.00
Client Reviews
4.9
Sitejabber
4.6
Trustpilot
4.8
Our Guarantees
100% Confidentiality
Information about customers is confidential and never disclosed to third parties.
Original Writing
We complete all papers from scratch. You can get a plagiarism report.
Timely Delivery
No missed deadlines – 97% of assignments are completed in time.
Money Back
If you're confident that a writer didn't follow your order details, ask for a refund.

Calculate the price of your order

You will get a personal manager and a discount.
We'll send you the first draft for approval by at
Total price:
$0.00
Power up Your Academic Success with the
Team of Professionals. We’ve Got Your Back.
Power up Your Study Success with Experts We’ve Got Your Back.

Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code ESSAYHELP