Analysis of a Professional Journal Article Submit Assignment

peer review an article

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

Analysis:
How to do it

Analysis is
written in memo format
State in the memo:
This memo provides an evaluation of an article from a professional journal. The analysis evaluates the visuals, mechanics, organization, and readability of the article as well as whether the author’s research shows evidence of pseudoscience. The analysis will also determine how the scientific method was used.

Analysis is
written in memo format
State in the memo:
The article was found by searching the online database [Name of database] using the keyword [xyz ]
I selected this article because I am pursuing a master’s degree in software engineering and am interested in [what is your interest?]
Overall it is a [good, adequate, etc. article,]
Does it have any weaknesses? 

Analysis -Audience
The author’s intended audience appears to be [interested in topic? Experts? But for those without a technical background, the article will be a challenge to read through.]
Through the use of visuals, a good format layout, and clear writing, the authors do a [good, fairly good, adequate] job in communicating

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

Analysis -Organization
The introduction of the article clearly states the subject, purpose, and scope of the article.
Each paragraph also has a topic sentence that provides a good introduction of the supporting details. For example, when the author discusses xyz, he starts with x, and continues to explain [x]

Analysis -Organization
However, the introduction does not provide a complete outline of the article. For example__the author leaves out information about______. The faults in the introduction provide the reader with a false impression of the flow of the article, possibly leading to confusion and misunderstanding.
Some terms the author mentions are/are not clearly defined. This makes the article more difficult/easier to understand.
Additionally, the article does not have a conclusion. But the article needs a conclusion to summarize important points found throughout the article.

Analysis -Readability
Despite problems with____the rest of the article is organized in a logical manner. (Note the connection to the previous section)
The logical flow makes it easy to read. The author has a unique way of illustrating and providing context for some of the technical terminology [or makes it difficult.]
For example…

Analysis -Formatting
Good formatting aids in providing a good flow for the readers.
Spacing, section headings, a good table of contents, glossary…
For example, the author…

Analysis –Grammar and Mechanics
Good grammar and mechanics also aid in making the article understandable. Why?
Grammatical mistakes are distracting and slow the reader down….
Mistakes also show a lack of credibility and professionalism…
Except for some rare cases in which the writing style shifted from formal to informal, the grammar and mechanics overall were fine…

Analysis –Scientific
Method and Pseudoscience
Although the grammar and mechanics are not exactly precise, the science methodology and data collection appear to be sound. For example…..
To address the research, it is evident the author used the scientific method. For example…
Because the author used the scientific method, there is no evidence of pseudoscience. In addition, the journal’s articles are peer reviewed, and the article would not have been published had the reviews suspected any pseudoscience.

Analysis – Conclusion
[Summarize by mentioning information from the above sections…]
The proper use of visuals, implementation of a professional scientific format, and good readability and formatting work together to create successful publication.
This experience helped me [in what way?] better understand research communication? A key lesson I learned was…

RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access

Avian influenza A/H7N9 risk perception,
information trust and adoption of
protective behaviours among poultry
farmers in Jiangsu Province, China
Bin Cui1,2, Qiuyan Liao3*, Wendy Wing Tak Lam3, Zong Ping Liu2,4 and Richard Fielding3

  • Abstract
  • Background
  • : Poultry farmers are at high-risk from avian influenza A/H7N9 infection due to sustained occupational
    exposures to live poultry. This study examined factors associated with poultry farmers’ adoption of personal
    protective behaviours (PPBs) based on Protection Motivation Theory (PMT).

  • Methods
  • : Totally, 297 poultry farmers in three cities of Jiangsu Province, China were interviewed during November
    2013-January 2014. Data on PMT constructs, perceived trustworthiness of A/H7N9 information from mass media
    (formal sources), friends and family (informal sources), intention to adopt and actual adoption of PPBs and
    respondents’ demographics were collected. Structural equation modeling (SEM) identified associations between
    demographic factors and PMT constructs associated with A/H7N9-oriented PPB intention. Moderated mediation
    analysis examined how demographics moderated the effects of information trust on PPB intention via risk
    perceptions of A/H7N9.

  • Results
  • : Respondents generally perceived low vulnerability to A/H7N9 infection. The SEM found that male
    respondents perceived lower severity of (β = −0.23), and lower vulnerability to (β = -0.15) A/H7N9 infection; age
    was positively associated with both perceived personal vulnerability to (β = 0.21) and perceived self-efficacy
    (β = 0.24) in controlling A/H7N9; education was positively associated with perceived response efficacy (β = 0.40).
    Furthermore, perceived vulnerability (β = 0.16), perceived self-efficacy (β = 0.21) and response efficacy (β = 0.67)
    were positively associated with intention to adopt PPBs against A/H7N9. More trust in informal information (TII) was
    only significantly associated with greater PPB intention through its positive association with perceived response
    efficacy. Age significantly moderated the associations of TII with perceived Self-efficacy and perceived response
    efficacy, with younger farmers who had greater TII perceiving lower self-efficacy but higher response efficacy.

    Conclusion: Poultry farmers perceive A/H7N9 as a personally-irrelevant risk. Interventions designed to enhance
    perceived response efficacy, particularly among lower educated respondents may effectively motivate adoption of
    PPBs. Informal information may be an important resource for enhancing response efficacy.

    Keywords: Influenza A (H7N9), Risk perception, Information trust, Behaviour, Poultry farmers

    * Correspondence: qyliao11@hku.hk
    3Division of Behavioural Sciences, School of Public Health, The University of
    Hong Kong, 21 Sassoon Road, Pokfulam, Hong Kong, Special Administrative
    Region, China
    Full list of author information is available at the end of the article

    © The Author(s). 2017 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
    International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
    reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to
    the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver
    (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

    Cui et al. BMC Public Health (2017) 17:463
    DOI 10.1186/s12889-017-4364-y

    http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1186/s12889-017-4364-y&domain=pdf

    mailto:qyliao11@hku.hk

    http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/

    http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/

    Background
    The first human cases of avian influenza A (H7N9) were
    reported in eastern China in March 2013 [1], subse-
    quently spreading to over 10 provinces in China [2]. For-
    tunately, the virus transmits inefficiently between
    humans [3, 4]. Genomic analysis suggests that A/H7N9
    virus is of avian origin and mainly transmitted through
    exposure to infected poultry [1]. The median age of
    confirmed A/H7N9 human cases was 61 years [3], indi-
    cating that older people are a high risk group for A/
    H7N9 infection. Over 60% of confirmed A/H7N9 human
    cases reported a history of exposure to live poultry [2].
    This raises concerns that those in frequent contact with
    poultry such as poultry traders and poultry farmers are
    at high risk of A/H7N9 infection. Although only 6% of
    confirmed A/H7N9 human cases were poultry workers
    [3], one previous study reported that over 50% of the
    surveyed poultry workers had seroconversion for A/
    H7N9 virus from May 2013 to December 2013 in South-
    ern China though none had virologically confirmed A/
    H7N9 infection [5]. This indicates that people with oc-
    cupational exposure to poultry could have a high risk of
    mild or asymptomatic A/H7N9 infection. More recently,
    one study indicates that poultry farms could be import-
    ant sources of reassortment between A/H7N9 virus and
    other strains of avian influenza viruses [6]. Therefore,
    poultry farmers may have a high risk of A/H7N9
    infection.
    The “China Animal Industry Yearbook 2011” reports

    China having at least 44,061,961 poultry farmers [7].
    Large-scale migration in 1990s China saw younger
    adults migrate from rural to urban areas becoming fac-
    tory workers [8], sharply raising the mean age of the
    remaining rural residents with proportions of residents
    greater than 60 years increasing from 10.9% in 2000 to
    15.0% in 2010 [9]. Therefore, many rural Chinese
    poultry farmers are probably older and potentially more
    vulnerable to A/H7N9 infection.
    Understanding how people at high-risk respond to the

    outbreak of this novel influenza can guide public health
    interventions. For example, previous studies identified
    that an erroneous belief that cooking was the best way
    of protection from avian influenza could reassure
    continuing buying of live poultry from wet markets the
    public [10] and that live poultry traders generally failed
    to recognize the risks from contact with bird secretions
    or droppings [11]. All these knowledge deficits could be
    addressed by public health education to improve pro-
    tective behaviours. How people perceive the risk of a
    novel influenza appears to partially influence their pro-
    tective behaviours [12–14]. However, although the rela-
    tionships between risk perception and self-protective
    behaviours have been widely examined in many descrip-
    tive studies following novel influenza outbreaks, many

    are atheoretical [14] and this limits the confidence we
    have in the veracity of the findings. A theoretical basis is
    important because it generates testable predictions that
    build confidence in the validity of the underlying pro-
    cesses. Studies suggest that protective behaviours in re-
    sponse to newly emerging infectious disease outbreaks
    differ by respondents’ socio-demographic characteristics,
    particularly age, gender and educational attainment [14]
    possibly because these variables influence perception of
    risk [15, 16]. However, few studies have tested these hy-
    potheses within any theoretical frameworks. Studies of
    pandemic influenza A/H1N1 found that older respon-
    dents perceived greater severity of, but lower personal
    susceptibility to the disease while males generally per-
    ceived lower severity of and personal susceptibility to
    the disease [15, 17]. Few studies reported the relation-
    ships between educational attainment and risk percep-
    tion of influenza. However, higher educational level has
    been consistently associated with lower perceived risk
    from other health threats [18, 19], possibly because
    higher educated people are more likely to be unrealistic-
    ally optimistic when evaluating their personal risk [19]
    which might imply greater personal agency or self-
    efficacy [20]. In relation to experience, farmers with
    more experience in raising poultry report more familiar-
    ity with poultry diseases and thereby perceived lower
    risk from avian influenza and higher confidence in pre-
    venting the disease [13, 21]. Consequently, the first ob-
    jective of this study was to examine how A/H7N9-
    related risk perceptions and demographics including
    age, gender, educational attainment and working
    experience (indicating by years of raising poultry) influ-
    enced intention to adopt personal protective behaviours
    (PPBs) against A/H7N9. We hypothesized that demo-
    graphics influence intention to adopt PPBs against A/
    H7N9 through their effects on A/H7N9-related risk
    perceptions.
    Sources of information are important when consider-

    ing threat-related information veracity. We distinguish
    between different information sources as follows: Learn-
    ing from the experience of the 2003 SARS outbreak in
    China, the Chinese government actively disseminated
    information about A/H7N9 through traditional mass
    media (e.g., TV, radio and newspaper) since it emerged
    in China in March 2013 [22–24]. Traditional mass
    media in Mainland China are mainly regarded as
    government-agency sources for information of infectious
    diseases and thereby such information is assumed to
    constitute “formal information” in this study. Informa-
    tion of A/H7N9 disseminated through media is likely to
    provoke widespread public discussion about the topic. In
    contrast, information communicated through casual
    interpersonal communication between friends and fam-
    ily constitutes “informal information” for the purposes of

    Cui et al. BMC Public Health (2017) 17:463 Page 2 of 13

    this study. Trust is a core element for effective risk com-
    munication, particularly for uncertain infection risks
    where the risk-related threat is usually invisible [25].
    Causal models of trust propose that information trust
    influences behavioural change indirectly through alter-
    ations in risk perceptions [26–28]. The literature on
    trust suggests two main types of trust can be identified;
    trust based on judgments of the intentions of others (re-
    lational trust) and the trust based on judgments of com-
    petence (calculative trust) [29]. While trust in informal
    information (TII) approximates to relational trust, trust
    in government (formal) information approximates to
    calculative trust [29]. Therefore, we propose that trust in
    formal and informal information may function di-
    fferentially to motivate behavioural change through their
    effects on risk perceptions [29]. Previous studies con-
    ducted among the general public during the 2009 influ-
    enza A/H1N1 pandemic suggests that while trust in
    formal information was significantly associated with
    perceived confidence in preventing the disease (efficacy
    appraisal), TII was significantly associated with perceived
    risk of the disease (threat appraisal) [30]. Furthermore,
    the degree of trust in health information from various
    sources differed by demographics including age, gender
    and educational attainment [31]. Therefore, it seems
    plausible that demographics including age, gender and
    education attainment exert their effects by modifying the
    effects of information trust on risk perceptions. Therefore,
    the second objective of the current study was to examine
    whether demographics including age, gender and educa-
    tional attainment could modify the indirect effects of in-
    formation trust on intention to take protective behaviours
    through risk perceptions related to A/H7N9. Due to lack
    of available data, no hypotheses about the direction of the
    moderated effects were set for this objective.

    Methods
    The theoretical framework
    This study was designed based on Protection Motivation
    Theory (PMT) which has been used successfully to pre-
    dict a variety of behaviours [32, 33]. Many studies have
    suggested that PMT provides a useful theoretical frame-
    work for understanding people’s response to threat-
    related information during outbreaks of newly-emerging
    respiratory infectious diseases [14, 33]. PMT focuses on
    individuals’ cognitive processes in response to fear ap-
    peal messages. It proposes that four core cognitive pro-
    cesses mediate the effects of fear appeal messages on
    motivation to adopt protective behaviours [32]. These
    four core cognitive processes are perceived Vulnerability
    (i.e. subjective estimates of the chance of contracting a
    disease), perceived Severity (i.e., subjective estimates of
    the seriousness of a disease), perceived Self-efficacy (i.e.,
    the belief that one can successfully take the preventive

    behaviours) and perceived Response Efficacy (i.e., the be-
    lief that existing preventive behaviours are effective in
    reducing risk of the disease) [34]. PMT also predicts that
    individual characteristics influence motivation for behav-
    ioural change through their effects on these four cogni-
    tive components [32]. In this study, we hypothesized
    that poultry farmers’ demographics, including gender,
    age, educational attainment and years of raising poultry
    influence the PMT constructs of perceived Vulnerability
    (to A/H7N9), perceived Severity (of A/H7N9), perceived
    Self-efficacy and perceived Response Efficacy in control-
    ling H7N9, which in turn influence poultry farmers’
    intention to adopt PPBs against A/H7N9. Figure 1 out-
    lines the conceptual model used. According to the hy-
    potheses of PMT, all the four core components, perceived
    Vulnerability, perceived Severity, perceived Self-efficacy
    and perceived Response Efficacy, are hypothesized to be
    positively associated with intention to adopt PPBs.
    Due to limited literature on the relationship between

    demographics variables and PMT constructs related to
    avian influenzas, we hypothesized a model comprising
    saturated relationships (testing all possible relationship
    permutations) between specified demographic variables
    and PMT constructs (Fig. 1). Drawing on prior studies
    we hypothesized that females would perceive higher
    Vulnerability to and higher Severity of A/H7N9 while
    older people would perceive lower Vulnerability to but
    higher Severity of A/H7N9; farmers with more years’
    experience of raising poultry would perceive lower Vul-
    nerability to A/H7N9 and lower Severity of A/H7N9
    infection, but perceive higher Self-efficacy and Response
    Efficacy in preventing A/H7N9. For other associations
    between demographics and PMT constructs, no hypoth-
    eses about the directions of associations were set due to
    lack of prior data.

    Sampling
    In the 2013–2014 A/H7N9 outbreak in Mainland China
    [35], around 52.3% of the cases were reported from
    Zhejiang, Jiangsu and Shanghai, three provinces located
    in eastern China along the Yangtze River delta. A total of
    59 A/H7N9 human cases had been reported as of
    December 31, 2014 in Jiangsu Province, with a fatality
    rate of around 29.6% [36]. Around 28.2% of all con-
    firmed cases of A/H7N9 between March 2013 and June
    2014 in China were farmers and around 6% were poultry
    framers or workers [37]. It was estimated that there were
    at least 1,094,505 poultry farmers in Jiangsu Province
    in 2011 [7]. This study was conducted in three cities
    of Jiangsu Province: Suqian, Nantong and Zhenjiang
    (Additional file 1: Figure S1).
    A/H7N9 virus has been isolated from various birds in-

    cluding pigeons, chickens and ducks [38] but viral shed-
    ding is higher and more prolonged in quails and

    Cui et al. BMC Public Health (2017) 17:463 Page 3 of 13

    chickens compared to other species [39]. Considering
    that type of poultry may be a factor that influences
    poultry farmers’ A/H7N9 risk perceptions and that
    chicken is the dominant type of poultry raised by these
    poultry farmers, this study only recruited poultry
    farmers who raised chickens.
    Subjects were recruited using a mixed strategy of

    stratified sampling and random sampling (Fig. 2). Firstly,
    three prefectural-level cities located in the northern,
    central and southern parts of Jiangsu Province respect-
    ively were selected. Within each selected prefectural-
    level city, two county-level cities were randomly selected
    from all those within the prefectural-level city, and
    within each county-level city, two county-level districts
    were randomly selected. Following this, three villages
    were randomly selected from each selected county-level
    district. Finally, around 10 poultry farmers within each
    selected village were randomly selected according to
    the name lists provided by local veterinary authorities

    (which must record all licensed poultry farms), and
    approached by the trained researcher for the face-to-
    face interview. A flow chart showing the process of
    sampling was provided (Fig. 2).

    Ethics, consent and permissions
    This study was conducted during November 2013 to
    January 2014 following ethical approval from the
    Yangzhou University and local veterinary bureau
    which is mainly responsible for distributing avian influ-
    enza prevention guideline and monitoring the poultry
    health and the health of people who work with poultry in
    Mainland China. The target subjects were first given an
    explanation of the study and then their consent to partici-
    pate was sought. Those agreeing completed a face-to-face
    interview using a standardized questionnaire. The ques-
    tionnaire was fully anonymous without collecting any
    personal identity information. Farmers who were not at
    home at the time when they were approached or refused

    Fig. 2 The flow chart showing the process of sampling

    Fig. 1 The conceptual framework based on Protection Motivation Theory for understanding farmers’ intention to adopt protective behaviours
    against avian influenza A/H7N9

    Cui et al. BMC Public Health (2017) 17:463 Page 4 of 13

    to participate were replaced with their nearest neighbor
    poultry farmers, again based on the veterinary authorities’
    lists. Each subject who completed the survey was pre-
    sented with a small gift (a towel and soap).

    Study instrument
    A questionnaire was designed to measure major con-
    structs of PMT including perceived Vulnerability to and
    perceived Severity of A/H7N9, perceived Self-efficacy
    and perceived Response Efficacy for protecting against
    A/H7N9 infection, the intention to adopt, and actual
    adoption of PPBs against A/H7N9 infection, degree of
    trust in information about A/H7N9 from traditional
    mass media (formal), family and friends (informal)
    sources and finally demographics including gender, age,
    education and the number of years spent raising poultry.
    Specifically, the measures for perceived Severity (4

    items), perceived Vulnerability (4 items), perceived Self-
    efficacy (4 items), perceived Response Efficacy (4 items),
    and protective intention (3 items) were adapted from
    earlier pre-validated studies [40–42]. For these items,
    the respondents were asked to indicate on a 7-point
    Likert-type scale their level of agreement or disagree-
    ment with each statement in the questionnaire (ranging
    from “1 = very strongly disagree” to “7 = very strongly
    agree”). The measure of actual PPBs included seven
    questions that asked respondents if they had adopted
    each of seven protective behaviours (wearing gloves,
    wearing protective clothes, wearing a face mask, wearing
    a protective hat, wearing protective shoes, washing
    hands after touching dead poultry, washing hands after
    touching poultry feces (Yes/No)) in their routine hus-
    bandry practices. Protective behaviour adoption was
    recorded as “1”. Otherwise, “0” was recorded. These
    seven protective behaviours are recommended by the
    National Health and Family Planning Commission of
    China in their proposal for personal protection against
    highly pathogenic avian influenza for high risk persons
    including the poultry workers and farmers, aiming to
    reduce their risk of contracting avian influenza viral
    infection due to occupational exposure to poultry [43].
    Two items, each assessing trust in information about A/
    H7N9 from formal (e.g., how much do you trust the in-
    formation about A/H7N9 influenza from newspaper, TV
    and radio?) and informal sources (e.g., how much do
    you trust the information about A/H7N9 influenza from
    you friends or relatives?), respectively, were measured
    with a 5-point Likert-type scale (ranging from “1 = do
    not believe” to “5 = fully believe”). Items for measuring
    the PMT constructs and basic descriptive data were
    shown in Additional file 2: Table S1.
    The questionnaire was pretested for its comprehen-

    sibility and length among 45 chicken farmers from a
    country of Suqian city in October 2013 before being

    formally used in the survey. Minor amendments were
    made for items that were not easily understood by the
    farmers but the original meanings of the items were
    retained.

    Data analysis
    To assess the reliability and validity of measures for the
    PMT constructs including perceived Vulnerability to and
    perceived Severity of A/H7N9 infection, perceived Self-
    efficacy and Response Efficacy for preventing A/H7N9
    and intention to adopt PPBs against A/H7N9, Cron-
    bach’s alpha (α) coefficients for each latent variable were
    first calculated. All α values exceeded 0.80 (Additional
    file 2: Table S2), indicating high internal consistency
    (internal reliability) for the measures [44]. Then, the
    average variance extracted (AVE) was used to assess the
    validity of all these scales. A value of AVE greater than
    0.5 for a latent variable indicates a good convergent
    validity for that variable [45]. The results showed that
    the AVE values of all PMT constructs exceeded 0.80,
    suggesting high convergent validity for these latent vari-
    ables. Using the Fornell–Larcker criterion, the square
    root of all AVE values (the diagonal values in Additional
    file 2: Table S2) were higher than the correlations
    between all latent variables (off-diagonal values) indicat-
    ing that each latent variable shares more variance with
    its assigned indicators than with any other latent vari-
    able. Such results suggest good discriminant validity for
    each latent variable.
    The conceptual model (Fig. 1) was tested using struc-

    tural equation modelling (SEM) with demographic
    variables entered into the model as observed covariant
    variables and PMT constructs entered as latent variables.
    All covariance, factor loadings, measurement errors, dis-
    turbances and path coefficients were estimated using
    robust maximum likelihood (MLR) estimator [45]. Path
    coefficients with p-values less than 0.05 were considered
    as statistically significant. Multiple model fit indices
    including CFI, TLI, RMSEA and SRMR was used to assess
    the global model fit. Values of CFI and TLI great than 0.9,
    of RMSEA and SRMR less than 0.8 suggest an acceptable
    fit of the model to data. The local fit of the model was
    assessed by investigating the residual matrix. Since the
    model was run with MLR estimator, the Satorra-Bentler
    scaled chi-square difference test [46] was used to compare
    nested models in order to identify the optimal and more
    parsimonious model. The direct effects of risk perceptions
    and indirect effects of demographic on Intention to adopt
    protective behaviours through risk perceptions were cal-
    culated using Bootstrapping methods.
    To assess whether the effects of information trust on

    Intention to take protective behaviours through A/
    H7N9-related risk perceptions could be modified by age,
    gender and educational attainment, two analytic steps

    Cui et al. BMC Public Health (2017) 17:463 Page 5 of 13

    were adopted. First, we tested the simple mediation
    model which hypothesized that the effect of information
    trust on Intention to take protective behaviours was me-
    diated by perceived Severity, perceived Vulnerability,
    perceived Self-efficacy and perceived Response efficacy.
    Once the simple mediation relationship was established,
    multiple group modelling was conducted to examine the
    conditional indirect effect for each moderator (i.e., gen-
    der, age and education). Bootstrapping methods were
    used to calculate the 95% confidence interval of specific
    conditional indirect effects. Significant difference in
    conditional indirect effects across stratum of the mod-
    erator indicates significant moderated effect on the me-
    diation relationship. All analyses were conducted using
    Mplus 7.0.

    Results
    The participants
    A total of 297 respondents were recruited from 360
    poultry farmers approached, a response rate of 82.5%.
    All respondents completed the face-to-face interview
    based on the questionnaire without missing data. These
    297 chicken farmers fed between 300 and 25,000 chick-
    ens (median = 4000) each. Policy changes in Jiangsu
    Province encourage large-scale poultry farming while
    discouraging small-scale backyard poultry husbandry in
    order to increase the management standards of rural
    poultry farming. Of the respondents, 76.1% were male,
    while 50.8% and 30.6% were aged 46–55 years and
    ≥56 years, respectively; 76.1% of the respondents
    attained junior high school or lower educational achieve-
    ment and over half (56.6%) had raised chickens for at
    least 10 years (Table 1).

    A/H7N9 risk perceptions, intention to adopt and actual
    adoption of PPBs against A/H7N9
    Respondents generally reported low perceived Vulnerabil-
    ity to A/H7N9 (mean value = 2.32 possible range 1–7)
    while perceived Severity of A/H7N9 was high (mean
    value = 5.96 possible range 1–7) (Additional file 2:
    Table S1). Perceived Self-efficacy was also high (mean
    value = 5.75 possible range 1–7) while perceived
    Response Efficacy (mean value = 4.81 possible range
    1–7) and intention to adopt PPBs against A/H7N9
    (mean value = 4.91 possible range 1–7) were moder-
    ate (Additional file 2: Table S1).
    Actual adherence to recommendations for washing

    hands after touching poultry feces (99.7%, 296/297),
    washing hands after touching dead poultry (89.9%, 267/
    297) and wearing protective clothing during poultry
    husbandry (87.9%, 261/297) were highly prevalent (Fig.
    3). Only one third of respondents (32.3%, 96/297) wore
    face masks during routine husbandry practices and
    20.5% (61/297) wore protective shoes. Around 12.5%
    (37/297) of the respondents adopted all the seven
    recommended protective behaviours. We ran a multi-
    variate logistic model to regress adoption of all seven
    recommended protective behaviours on age, gender,
    educational attainment and years of raising poultry. The
    results showed that after adjustment for other de-
    mographics, respondents who had higher educational
    attainment were more likely to adopt all the seven
    recommended protective behaviours (Reference group:
    Primary or below; OR = 10.06, 95%CI: 2.08-48.62 for
    junior high school; OR = 9.53, 95%CI: 1.68-54.04 for
    senior high school or above), while respondents who
    had raised poultry for 10–20 years were less likely to
    adopt all the seven recommended protective behaviours
    (OR = 0.04, 95%CI: 0.01-0.20) relative to respondents
    who had raised poultry for less than 10 years.

    Relationships among demographics, PMT constructs and
    intention to adopt PPBs
    The hypothesized model was initially run with all paths
    from demographic variables to PMT constructs freely es-
    timated (Model I). Then all insignificant paths (p ≥ 0.05)
    from the demographics to the PMT constructs were re-
    moved and the revised model (Model II) was re-run.
    Compared with Model I, Model II had comparable
    model fit indices, and the Satorra-Benter scaled chi-
    square difference test suggests that it fits to the data as
    well as Model I (Table 2). However, investigation of the
    residual matrix identified significant residual covariance
    between perceived Vulnerability and perceived Self-
    efficacy, between perceived Vulnerability and perceived
    Response Efficacy, between perceived Self-efficacy and
    perceived Severity, and between perceived Self-efficacy
    and perceived Response Efficacy. It indicates that the

    Table 1 Respondents’ characteristics (N = 297)

    Characteristics N %

    Gender

    Female 71 23.9%

    Male 226 76.1%

    Age

    ≦45 years 55 18.5%

    46-55 years 151 50.8%

    > 55 years 91 30.6%

    Education

    primary or below 73 24.6%

    Junior high school 153 51.5%

    Senior high school or above 71 23.9%

    Years raising poultry

    ≦10 years 129 43.4%

    10- 20 years 123 41.4%

    > 20 years 45 15.2%

    Cui et al. BMC Public Health (2017) 17:463 Page 6 of 13

    residual covariance of these latent variables is correlated.
    Therefore, the covariance between these variables was
    added to the model. The re-specified model (Model III) fits
    the data significantly better than Model II (Table 2). Investi-
    gation of the residual matrix revealed no significant residual
    covariance between the variables of Model III. Therefore,
    Model III was determined to be the optimal model.
    Compared with Model II, the parameters estimated for the
    structural part of Model III only differ slightly. The correla-
    tions between the four demographics age, gender, educa-
    tional attainment and years of raising poultry were not
    higher than 0.33 (spearman correlation between age and
    gender), suggesting that multicollinearity is not a significant
    problem of the model [47]. The standardized covariance,
    path coefficients and the explained variance of each en-
    dogenous variable for Model III are shown in Fig. 4.
    Gender was significantly associated with perceived Sever-

    ity (β = −0.23) and perceived Vulnerability (β = -0.15), with
    female respondents perceiving higher Severity of, and Vul-
    nerability to A/H7N9 infection than did males (Fig. 4). Age
    was significantly and positively associated with both per-
    ceived Vulnerability to A/H7N9 (β = 0.21) and perceived
    Self-efficacy (β = 0.24) in controlling A/H7N9. Educational
    attainment was only significantly and positively associated

    with perceived Response Efficacy (β = 0.40) while years of
    raising poultry were negatively associated with perceived
    Response Efficacy (β = -0.10). Subsequently, perceived
    Vulnerability to A/H7N9 (β = 0.16), perceived Self-efficacy
    (β = 0.21) and Response Efficacy (β = 0.67) were positively
    associated with Intention to adopt protective behaviours
    against A/H7N9. However, perceived Severity of A/H7N9
    was not significantly associated with protective Intention,
    which is inconsistent with PMT predictions. The model
    explained 50.7% of the variance in Intention to adopt PPBs
    but only explained 5.5%, 6.4%, 5.7% and 17.0% Perceived
    Severity, Perceived Vulnerability, Perceived Self-efficacy
    and Perceived Response Efficacy, respectively.
    The unstandardized direct effects of risk perceptions,

    and indirect effects of demographics via risk perceptions
    on Intention to adopt PPBs including the point estimate
    and 95% Bootstrapping confidence interval are shown in
    Table 3. Perceived Response Efficacy had strongest effect
    on behavioural Intention (point estimate = 0.54, 95%CI:
    0.47-0.63). While education (point estimate = 0.35, 95%CI:
    0.27-0.45) and age (point estimate = 0.11, 95%CI:
    0.05-0.18) had significant positive indirect effects, gender
    and years of raising poultry did not had significant indirect
    effects on behavioural Intention (Table 3).

    Fig. 3 Actual adoption of personal protective behaviours against A/H7N9 among the respondents

    Table 2 Comparison of model fit indices of Model I, Model II and Model III

    Nested models χ2 (df) Scaling correction factor CFI TLI RMSEA (90% CI) χ2 difference test (p)

    Model I 495.03 (148) 1.12 0.94 0.93 0.09 (0.08-0.10) –

    Model II 499.87 (158) 1.12 0.94 0.93 0.08 (0.08-0.09) p>0.10

    Model III 420.69 (154) 1.13 0.96 0.95 0.08 (0.07-0.08) p<0.001

    Model II is nested within Model I and Model III
    Compared with Model I, Model II removed the paths from gender to Perceived Self-efficacy and Perceived Response efficacy, from Age to Perceived Severity and
    Perceived Response efficacy, from Education to Perceived Severity, Perceived Vulnerability and Perceived Self-efficacy, and from years of raising poultry to
    Perceived Severity, Perceived Vulnerability and Perceived Response efficacy
    Compared with Model II, Mode III added covariance for the relationships of Perceived Vulnerability with Perceived Self-efficacy and Perceived Response efficacy,
    and the relationships of Perceived Self-efficacy with Perceived Severity and Perceived Response efficacy

    Cui et al. BMC Public Health (2017) 17:463 Page 7 of 13

    Fig. 4 The results of structural equation model for understanding determinants on intention to adopt protective behaviours against A/H7N9
    based on Protection Motivation Theory. a p<,0.05, b p < 0.01, c p < 0.001. The numbers on the paths are standardized path coefficient; the dotted line indicates the effect is not statistically significant

    Table 3 The direct effects of risk perceptions and indirect effects of demographics on Intention to adopt personal protective behaviours
    via risk perceptions

    Effects by exogenous variables

    Point estimate (SE) Bootstrapping (95%CI)

    Lower Upper

    Direct effects

    Perceived Severity→Intention -0.03 (0.03) -0.08 0.03

    Perceived Vulnerability→Intention 0.11 (0.04)b 0.04 0.18

    Perceived Self-efficacy→Intention 0.40 (0.09)c 0.23 0.58

    Perceived Response Efficacy→Intention 0.54 (0.04)c 0.47 0.63

    Indirect effects

    Gender→Intention

    Via Perceived Severity 0.02 (0.02) -0.02 0.06

    Via Perceived Vulnerability -0.05a -0.11 −0.01

    Total -0.03 (0.03) -0.10 0.02

    Age→Intention

    Via Perceived Vulnerability 0.04 (0.02)a 0.01 0.09

    Via Perceived Self-efficacy 0.07 (0.02)b 0.03 0.12

    Total 0.11 (0.03)c 0.05 0.18

    Education→Intention

    Via Perceived Response
    Efficacy

    0.35 (0.04)c 0.27 0.45

    Years of raising→Intention

    Via Perceived Response
    Efficacy

    -0.08 (0.04) -0.17 -0.00

    ap < 0.05, bp < 0.01, cp < 0.001; SE: Standard Error

    Cui et al. BMC Public Health (2017) 17:463 Page 8 of 13

    Information trust and the moderated effects of
    demographics on the relationships between information
    trust and PMT constructs
    As shown in Table 4, over 99% (275/297) of respondents
    indicated mostly or completely trusting information from
    formal sources (TV, radio or newspaper). In contrast, only
    14.5% (43/297) of respondents reported mostly or com-
    pletely trusting informal information (information from
    friends or relatives). Male respondents and those with
    higher educational achievement were more likely to trust in
    formal or informal information compared with their coun-
    terparts (Table 4). Information trust did not differ by age
    and years of raising poultry. The universally high level of
    trust in formal information complicates testing for the
    moderated mediation models due to almost zero data

    variability. Therefore, the analysis only focused on the mod-
    erated effects of demographics on the relationships of TII
    with behavioural Intention via risk perceptions of A/H7N9.
    The simple mediation model which hypothesized that

    effect of TII on Intention to adopt PPBs was mediated by
    perceived Severity, perceived Vulnerability, perceived Self-
    efficacy and perceived Response Efficacy were first tested.
    The simple mediation model fit well to the data (CFI=0.97,
    TLI=0.97, RMSEA=0.08 (90%CI: 0.07-0.09)). The results
    (Table 5) showed that only the indirect effects of TII on
    Intention through perceived Response Efficacy was signifi-
    cant (point estimate=0.57, 95%CI: 0.39-0.77).
    Then multiple group modelling with each moderator

    being treated as a grouping variable was conducted to cal-
    culate and compare the indirect effects of TII on Intention

    Table 4 Trust in formal and informal information by demographic characteristics

    Demographic characteristics Trust in formal information Trust in informal information

    Trust (mostly/completely trustworthy) P-valuea Trust (mostly/completely trustworthy) P-valueb

    Gender

    Female (69/71) 97.2% 0.011 (4/71) 5.6% 0.015

    Male (226/226)100% (39/226) 17.3%

    Age

    ≦45 years (55/55) 100% 0.804 (4/55) 7.3% 0.241

    46-55 years (149/151) 98.7% (24/151) 15.9%

    ≧56 years (91/91) 100% (15/91) 16.5%

    Education

    Primary or below (71/73) 97.3% 0.043 (7/73) 9.6% <0.001

    Junior high school (153/153) 100% (14/153) 9.2%

    Senior high school or above (71/71) 100% (22/71) 31.0%

    Years raising poultry

    ≦10 years (127/129) 98.4% 0.153 (12/129) 9.3% 0.080

    ≦20 years (123/123) 100% (22/123) 17.9%

    ≧21 years (45/45) 100% (9/45) 20.0%
    aFisher Exact test
    bPearson chi-square

    Table 5 The direct and indirect effects of trust in informal information on Intention to adopt personal protective behaviours based
    on the simple mediation model

    Point estimate (SE) Bootstrapping (95%CI)
    Lower Upper

    Direct effect -0.04 (0.13) -0.28 0.24

    Indirect effect

    Via perceived Severity -0.01 (0.01) -0.04 0.01

    Via perceived Susceptibility 0.01 (0.02) -0.02 0.07

    Via perceived Self-efficacy -0.07 (0.04) -0.17 -0.00

    Via perceived Response Efficacy 0.57 (0.10)c 0.39 0.77

    Total indirect effect 0.50 (0.09)c 0.33 0.69

    Total effect 0.46 (0.16)b 0.15 0.75
    b p<0.01, c p<0.001

    Cui et al. BMC Public Health (2017) 17:463 Page 9 of 13

    via risk perceptions of A/H7N9 (Table 6). It shows that the
    indirect effects of TII on Intention via perceived Severity
    and Vulnerability were not significant across stratum of
    gender, age group and educational achievement. The indir-
    ect effect of TII on Intention via perceived Self-efficacy was
    only significant for female (point estimate=-0.24, 95%CI:
    -0.52–-0.06) and younger farmers (point estimate=-0.16,
    95%CI: -0.30–-0.07). Age significantly moderated the medi-
    ation of TII with Intention via perceived Self-efficacy, with
    younger farmers who had more trust in informal informa-
    tion perceived lower self-efficacy. The indirect effects of TII
    on Intention via perceived Response Efficacy were signifi-
    cant across stratum of gender, age group and educational
    achievement excepting for framers who were older than 55
    years. Age significantly moderated the mediation of TII
    with Intention via perceived Response Efficacy, with youn-
    ger farmers who had more trust in informal information
    perceived higher response efficacy.

  • Discussion
  • Based on PMT, our study investigated how cognitive
    processes mediated the effects of demographics on
    motivation to adopt protective behaviours against A/
    H7N9, and how information trust interacted with demo-
    graphics to influence A/H7N9 protection among the
    Chinese poultry farmers.
    Generally, the study found that the respondents

    perceived A/H7N9 infection to be severe but did not per-
    ceive themselves to be vulnerable to the infection. This is
    consistent with one previous study conducted in The
    Netherlands which found that over 90% of the respondents
    perceived that avian influenza was a serious disease (mean
    score = 4.57, scale 1–5) but only 0.7% of them perceived
    themselves to be highly vulnerable to avian influenza (mean
    score = 1.69, scale 1–5) [48]. Chinese poultry farmers
    report more familiarity with poultry disease risk than do
    urbanites and are more optimistic about avoiding avian
    influenzas [12, 13]. Both familiarity and optimistic bias
    probably further account for the low perceived Vulnerabil-
    ity observed among these Jiangsu poultry farmers.

    All PMT constructs were positively associated with
    PPB intention except for perceived Severity of A/H7N9
    which was not significantly associated with PPB intention.
    The meta-analysis on the efficacy of PMT also indicates
    that the effect size of perceived Severity on protection
    motivation was the smallest among the four PMT
    constructs [32]. Given the small-to-moderate effect size of
    perceived Severity on behavioral intention, our small
    sample size may not be able to detect a significant
    association. However, while a previous review indicated that
    Self-efficacy had the strongest effect on behavioral intention
    [32], our study found that perceived Response Ef-
    ficacy had the strongest effect on PPB intention,
    accounting for nearly 50% of the explained variance
    in PPB intention. For these poultry farmers, perceived
    Self-efficacy to adopt the preventive measures was gener-
    ally high possibly because the recommended preventive
    measures are simple and thereby easily adopted. In this
    case, whether the preventive measures are believed to
    be effective or not to reduce risk of A/H7N9 plays a
    dominant role in determining their motivation to
    adopt the measures.
    The finding that respondents with higher educational

    achievement had better compliance to PPBs is consistent
    with a previous study reporting better educated poultry
    traders were more likely to adopt PPBs when working [49].
    Our study adds to the literature about the potential mech-
    anism of how education influence adoption of PPBs. As in-
    dicated by the SEM, better educated respondents perceived
    higher response efficacy to prevent A/H7N9 which in turn
    was associated with higher intention to adopt protective be-
    haviours against A/H7N9. Compared with other de-
    mographics, education had stronger indirect effects on
    intention to adopt protective behaviours via Perceived Re-
    sponse Efficacy. This suggests that interventions to pro-
    mote belief in the efficacy of available protective behaviours
    among the less educated farmers may play a crucial role to
    improve compliance to self-protection against A/H7N9.
    Consistent with our hypotheses, our study also found

    that females perceived higher personal Vulnerability to

    Table 6 The estimated conditional indirect effects of trust in informal information on intention to adopt personal protective
    behaviours against influenza A/H7N9 via risk perceptions

    Moderator Level Conditional indirect effects of TII on Intention (Bootstrapping 95% CI) via:

    Perceived Severity Perceived Vulnerability Perceived Self-efficacy Perceived Response efficacy

    Gender Female 0.04 (-0.03, 0.13) -0.16 (-0.39, 0.10) -0.24 (-0.52, -0.06)a 0.30 (0.16-0.49)b

    Male -0.02 (-0.08, 0.00) 0.05 (0.00, 0.15) -0.05 (-0.17, 0.00) 0.56 (0.33, 0.80)c

    Age group (years) ≦55 -0.01 (-0.04, 0.01) 0.02 (-0.02, 0.07) -0.16 (-0.30, -0.07)b 0.77 (0.53, 1.00)c

    >55 -0.02 (-0.14, 0.04) -0.03 (-0.21, 0.11) 0.05 (-0.03, 0.19) 0.19 (-0.10, 0.44)

    Education Junior middle or below -0.02 (-0.08, 0.01) -0.01 (-0.09, 0.06) 0.08 (-0.01, 0.26) 0.25 (0.01, 0.41)a

    Senior high or above -0.00 (-0.07, 0.05) 0.08 (0.01, 0.29) 0.04 (-0.16, 0.27) 0.54 (0.23, 0.91)b

    a p<0.05, b p<0.01, c p<0.001; SE: Standard Error The bold values indicate that effects were significant different across stratum of a moderator

    Cui et al. BMC Public Health (2017) 17:463 Page 10 of 13

    A/H7N9 and higher Severity of A/H7N9 compared with
    males. This finding may elucidate why compliance to
    recommended protection was usually higher among
    females during epidemics found in many descriptive
    studies [14]. However, the associations between age and
    PMT constructs were not consistent with our hypoth-
    eses. According to the SEM, older respondents perceived
    higher Vulnerability to A/H7N9 and higher Self-efficacy
    to prevent against A/H7N9 which in turn was signifi-
    cantly associated with higher intention to adopt PPBs.
    However, our study did not find a significant association
    between age and compliance to the recommended pro-
    tective measures. This suggests factors other than these
    four PMT constructs may hinder translating the intention
    of adopting PPBs into actual behaviours among the older
    respondents. For example, perceived costs from taking the
    recommended behaviours (e.g., effort, time) may be
    greater among older people (e.g. older people need to take
    greater effort to alter their long-term habit) [32].
    Our initial hypotheses that years of working with

    poultry could be associated with lower perceived Vulner-
    ability and perceived Severity but higher perceived Self-
    efficacy and Response Efficacy were not supported. Years
    of working with poultry were only marginally associated
    with perceived Response Efficacy but the effect size
    was small. One possible reason could be that our
    measure of years of working with poultry may not be
    a good indicator for farmer’s experience with raising
    poultry. While a previous study did not find significant
    association between years of working with poultry and
    adoption of protective behaviours [11], our study found that
    respondents who had raised poultry for 10–20 years were
    less likely to adopt all the recommended protective mea-
    sures than those who had raised poultry for less than
    10 years after adjusting for age and educational attainment.
    Further studies are needed to explore the relationship
    between experience with poultry and adoption of PPBs
    among poultry farmers.
    The indirect effect of TII on Intention to adopt PPBs

    was only significant via perceived Response Efficacy,
    with greater TII being associated with greater perceived
    Response Efficacy which was positively associated with
    behavioural Intention. This is not consistent with one
    previous study that TII was independent of efficacy be-
    lief but was positively associated with disease worry [30].
    A possible reason for the inconsistent results could be
    that the former study was conducted among general public
    [30] while the current study was conducted among poultry
    farmers. Different types of informal information are prob-
    ably communicated among different populations. Farmers
    may simply know what kinds of protective behaviours are
    available for preventing A/H7N9 through listening to their
    peer farmers and observing what they do. Age significantly
    moderated the mediation relationships of TII with

    behavioural Intention through perceived Self-efficacy and
    Response Efficacy. For younger farmers, more trust in in-
    formation from peers was associated with perceived lower
    Self-efficacy but higher Response Efficacy, while corre-
    sponding associations were not statistically significant for
    older farmers. Such findings provide some insights about
    the possible types of information shared among younger
    poultry farmers. Younger poultry farmers who are usually
    better educated may be more aware of the effectiveness of
    available preventive measures in reducing risk of A/H7N9
    but they may lack of confidence in adopting these prevent-
    ive measures due to lack of skills in routine husbandry
    practices with protective measures. For example, they may
    find it more inconvenient to wear gloves or protective
    clothes in their routine husbandry practice. Therefore, it
    may be important to provide training for improving young
    poultry farmers’ skills of taking protective measures.
    This study has several limitations. First, the cross-

    sectional design excluded causal inference. Second, with-
    out follow-up data, this study cannot examine the gap
    between intention and subsequent adoption of PPBs,
    though current adherence to PPBs was measured, and
    past behaviour is the best predictor for future behaviours
    [50]. Third, actual PPBs were dichotomously (yes/no)
    measured and because of social desirability bias, may
    provide less accurate assessment of actual compliance.
    Furthermore, while multilevel SEM (MSEM) may be
    more appropriate for our data that were collected based
    on sampling stratified by clusters, MSEM cannot be
    conducted due to lack of data on clusters. Therefore,
    our single-level SEM assuming that subjects were in-
    dependent within clusters may underestimate the
    sampling variance, which may result in inflation of
    the type I error [45].

  • Conclusions
  • Jiangsu poultry farmers generally perceived A/H7N9
    Severity as high, but personal Vulnerability to infection
    as low, these variables being weakly associated with
    intention to adopt PPBs, possibly due to perceived
    personally-irrelevant risk. The moderate perceived Re-
    sponse Efficacy of respondents and its strongest associ-
    ation with PPB intention reflect that interventions
    designed to enhance perceived Response Efficacy may
    effectively motivate adoption of PPBs among these poultry
    farmers. Education appears to influence intention to adopt
    PPBs through its positive association with Response
    Efficacy, suggesting that Response Efficacy should be
    promoted among lower educated farmers. For example,
    information about how and why a recommended behavior
    can eliminate or decrease risk of infection should be
    clearly presented and framed in an easily-understood way
    for lower educated framers. The study also adds to the
    literature that gender appears to influence on intention to

    Cui et al. BMC Public Health (2017) 17:463 Page 11 of 13

    adopt PPBs through its effects on perceived disease Vul-
    nerability and Severity, while age may influence on behav-
    ioural intention through its effects on perceived disease
    Vulnerability and perceived Self-efficacy in prevention.
    Greater TII was associated with higher Intention to take
    protective measures through its positive association with
    perceived Response Efficacy. Age significantly moderated
    the association between TII and perceived Self-efficacy,
    and between TII and perceived Response Efficacy, with
    younger farmers who had greater TII perceived lower
    Self-efficacy but higher Response Efficacy. Young poultry
    farmers may just simply obtain the information about the
    availability of effective preventive measures against A/
    H7N9 from listening to what their peer farmers say and
    observing what they do. This suggests that interventions
    utilizing farmer peers to communicate and train poultry
    farmers in taking protective measures during routine hus-
    bandry practice may be effective to promote adoption of
    PPBs among poultry farmers.

  • Additional files
  • Additional file 1: Figure S1. Map of Jiangsu Province showing the
    sampling sites. Note: Maps of China and Jiangsu Province were
    reproduced based on maps provided by WIKIPEDIA available from
    https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Jiangsu (TIFF 1511 kb).

    Additional file 2: Table S1. and S2. The measuring items for the
    constructs of Protection Motivation Theory and descriptive statistics
    (DOCX 25 kb).

    Additional file 3: Data for analysis (XLS 163 kb).

  • Abbreviations
  • AVE: Average variance extracted; PMT: Protection Motivation Theory;
    PPBs: Personal protective behaviours; SEM: Structural equation modeling;
    TII: Trust in Informal Information

  • Acknowledgements
  • Not applicable.

  • Availability of data and materials
  • The data for this paper are available in the Additional file 3.

  • Funding
  • This research was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation of
    China (Grant no: 71,573,221).

  • Authors’ contributions
  • BC designed the study, collected the data, analyzed the data and
    drafted the manuscript. QL analyzed the data, guided data
    interpretation, drafted and revised the manuscript. WWTL advised on
    data interpretation and revised the manuscript. ZPL participated in the
    study design and coordination, and revised the manuscript. RF advised
    on data analysis and data interpretation, and revised the manuscript.
    All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

  • Competing interests
  • The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

  • Consent for publication
  • Not applicable.

  • Ethics approval and consent to participate
  • This study received ethical approval from the Yangzhou University and
    local veterinary bureau which is mainly responsible for monitoring the
    poultry health and health of the people who work with poultry in
    Mainland China. All participants gave oral consent to participate in the
    study before the interview started.

  • Publisher’s Note
  • Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in
    published maps and institutional affiliations.

  • Author details
  • 1Business College, Yangzhou University, Jiangsu Province, China. 2Jiangsu
    Co-innovation Center for Prevention and Control of Important Animal
    Infectious Diseases and Zoonoses, Yangzhou, China. 3Division of Behavioural
    Sciences, School of Public Health, The University of Hong Kong, 21 Sassoon
    Road, Pokfulam, Hong Kong, Special Administrative Region, China. 4College
    of Veterinary medicine, Yangzhou University, Jiangsu Province, People’s
    Republic of China.

    Received: 28 January 2016 Accepted: 3 May 2017

  • References
  • 1. Gao R, Cao B, Hu Y, Feng Z, Wang D, Hu W, Chen J, Jie Z, Qiu H, Xu K, et al.

    Human infection with a novel avian-origin influenza a (H7N9) virus. N Engl J
    Med. 2013;368:1888–97.

    2. Cowling BJ, Jin L, Lau EH, Liao Q, Wu P, Jiang H, Tsang TK, Zheng J, Fang VJ,
    Chang Z, et al. Comparative epidemiology of human infections with avian
    influenza a H7N9 and H5N1 viruses in China: a population-based study of
    laboratory-confirmed cases. Lancet. 2013;382(9887):129–37.

    3. Li Q, Zhou L, Zhou M, Chen Z, Li F, Wu H, Xiang N, Chen E, Tang F, Wang
    D, et al. Epidemiology of human infections with avian influenza a(H7N9)
    virus in China. N Engl J Med. 2014;370(6):520–32.

    4. Qi X, Qian Y-H, Bao C-J, Guo X-L, Cui L-B, Tang F-Y, Ji H, Huang Y, Cai P-Q,
    Lu B, et al. Probable person to person transmission of novel avian influenza
    a (H7N9) virus in eastern China, 2013: epidemiological investigation. BMJ.
    2013;347:f4752.

    5. Wang X, Fang S, Lu X, Xu C, Cowling BJ, Tang X, Peng B, Wu W, He J, Tang
    Y, et al. Seroprevalence to avian influenza a(H7N9) virus among poultry
    workers and the general population in southern China: a longitudinal study.
    Clin Infect Dis. 2014;59(6):e76–83.

    6. Wu D, Zou S, Bai T, Li J, Zhao X, Yang L, Liu H, Li X, Yang X, Xin L, et al.
    Poultry farms as a source of avian influenza a (H7N9) virus reassortment and
    human infection. Sci Rep. 2015;5:7630.

    7. Liu SZ, Li XR, Chen WS. China animal industry yearbook: 2012 (in Chinese).
    Beijing: Chinese Agricultural Press; 2012.

    8. Fan CC. China on the move: migration, the state, and the household. China
    Q. 2008;196:924–56.

    9. Zou XJ, Dan WU. Floating population impact on rural aging. Popul J. 2013;
    35(4):70–9.

    10. Fielding R, Lam WWT. Reducing avian influenza risk: a qualitative exploration of
    issues in Hong Kong. Health Educ. 2007;107(5):437–47.

    11. Ma XW, Liao Q, Yuan J, Liu YF, Liu YH, Chen JD, Liu JP, Cai WF, Cowling BJ,
    Di B, et al. Knowledge, attitudes and practices relating to influenza a(H7N9)
    risk among live poultry traders in Guangzhou City, China. BMC Infect Dis.
    2014;14:554.

    12. Liao Q, Lam WW, Jiang CQ, Ho EY, Liu YM, Zhang WS, Fielding R. Avian
    influenza risk perception and live poultry purchase in Guangzhou, China,
    2006. Risk Anal. 2009;29(3):416–24.

    13. Liao QY, Lam WW, Dang VT, Jiang CQ, Udomprasertgul V, Fielding R. What
    causes H5N1 avian influenza? Lay perceptions of H5N1 aetiology in south
    East and East Asia. J Public Health (Oxf). 2009;31(4):573–81.

    14. Bish A, Michie S. Demographic and attitudinal determinants of
    protective behaviours during a pandemic: a review. Br J Health Psychol.
    2010;15(Pt 4):797–824.

    15. Durham DP, Casman EA, Albert SM. Deriving behavior model parameters
    from survey data: self-protective behavior adoption during the 2009–2010
    influenza a(H1N1) pandemic. Risk Anal. 2012;32(12):2020–31.

    Cui et al. BMC Public Health (2017) 17:463 Page 12 of 13

    dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12889-017-4364-y

    https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Jiangsu

    dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12889-017-4364-y

    dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12889-017-4364-y

    16. Marris C, Langford I, Saunderson T, O’Riordan T. Exploring the “psychometric
    paradigm”: comparisons between aggregate and individual analyses. Risk
    Anal. 1997;17(3):303–12.

    17. Prati G, Pietrantoni L, Zani B. A social-cognitive model of pandemic
    influenza H1N1 risk perception and recommended behaviors in Italy. Risk
    Anal. 2011;31(4):645–56.

    18. Essien EJ, Ogungbade GO, Ward D, Ekong E, Ross MW, Meshack A, Holmes
    LJ. Influence of educational status and other variables on human
    immunodeficiency virus risk perception among military personnel: a large
    cohort finding. Mil Med. 2007;172(11):1177–81.

    19. Peterson LM, Helweg-Larsen M, Volpp KG, Kimmel SE. Heart attack risk
    perception biases among hypertension patients: the role of educational
    level and worry. Psychol Health. 2012;27(6):737–51.

    20. Sherer M, Maddux JE, Mercandante B, Prentice-Dunn S, Jacobs B, Rogers
    RW. The self-efficacy scale: construction and validation. Psychol Rep. 1982;
    51(2):663–71.

    21. Fielding R, Lam WW, Ho EY, Lam TH, Hedley AJ, Leung GM. Avian influenza
    risk perception, Hong Kong. Emerg Infect Dis. 2005;11(5):677–82.

    22. Fung IC-H, Wong K. Efficient use of social media during the avian
    influenza a(H7N9) emergency response. West Pac Surveill Response J:
    WPSAR. 2013;4(4):1–3.

    23. Vong S, O’leary M, Feng Z. Early response to the emergence of influenza
    A(H7N9) virus in humans in China: the central role of prompt information
    sharing and public communication. Bull World Health Organ. 2014;92:303–8.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.2471/BLT.13.125989.

    24. Xie T, Yang Z, Yang S, Wu N, Li L. Correlation between reported human
    infection with avian influenza a H7N9 virus and cyber user awareness: what
    can we learn from digital epidemiology? Int J Infect Dis. 2014;22:1–3.

    25. Calman KC. Communication of risk: choice, consent, and trust. Lancet. 2002;
    360(9327):166–8.

    26. Siegrist M. The influence of trust and perceptions of risks and benefits on
    the acceptance of gene technology. Risk Anal. 2000;20(2):195–203.

    27. Siegrist M, Cvetkovich G, Roth C. Salient value similarity, social trust, and
    risk/benefit perception. Risk Anal. 2000;20(3):353–62.

    28. Bronfman NC, Vazquez EL. A cross-cultural study of perceived benefit
    versus risk as mediators in the trust-acceptance relationship. Risk Anal.
    2011;31(12):1919–34.

    29. Earle TC. Trust in risk management: a model-based review of empirical
    research. Risk Anal. 2010;30(4):541–74.

    30. Liao Q, Cowling B, Lam WT, Ng MW, Fielding R. Situational awareness and
    health protective responses to pandemic influenza a (H1N1) in Hong Kong:
    a cross-sectional study. PLoS One. 2010;5(10):e13350.

    31. Smith D. Health care consumer’s use and trust of health information
    sources. J Commun Healthc. 2011;4(3):200–10.

    32. Floyd DL, Prentice-Dunn S, Rogers RW. A meta-analysis of research on
    protection motivation theory. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2000;30(2):407–29.

    33. Ruiter RAC, Kessels LTE, Peters G-JY, Kok G. Sixty years of fear appeal
    research: current state of the evidence. Int J Psychol. 2014;49(2):63–70.

    34. Rogers RW, Prentice-Dunn S. Handbook of health behavior research 1:
    personal and social determinants. New York, NY, US: Plenum Press; 1997.

    35. Xu J, Huang S, Xiao W, Hu J, Sun H. Spatial aggregation of 438 human
    infections with avian influenza a (H7N9) in the mainland of China.
    Zhonghua Liu Xing Bing Xue Za Zhi. 2014;35(11):1270–4.

    36. Wang L, Zeng L, Ren X, Geng M, Li Z, Yu H. Analysis of morbidity and
    mortality characteristics of the notifiable diseases reported in 2013 in China.
    Zhonghua Liu Xing Bing Xue Za Zhi. 2015;36(3):194–8.

    37. Ren R, Zhou L, Xiang N, Liu B, Zhao J, Geng X, Wang Y, Li C, Lyu Y,
    Yang F, et al. Epidemiological characteristics of human avian influenza a
    (H7N9) virus infection in China. Zhonghua Liu Xing Bing Xue Za Zhi.
    2014;35(12):1362–5.

    38. Guan Y, Farooqui A, Zhu H, Dong W, Wang J, Kelvin DJ. H7N9 incident,
    immune status, the elderly and a warning of an influenza pandemic. J Infect
    Dev Ctries. 2013;7(4):302–7.

    39. Pantin-Jackwood MJ, Miller PJ, Spackman E, Swayne DE, Susta L, Costa-
    Hurtado M, Suarez DL. Role of poultry in the spread of novel H7N9
    influenza virus in China. J Virol. 2014;88(10):5381–90.

    40. Lee Y. Understanding anti-plagiarism software adoption: an extended
    protection motivation theory perspective. Decis Support Syst. 2011;
    50(2):361–9.

    41. Milne S, Sheeran P, Orbell S. Prediction and intervention in health-related
    behavior: a meta-analytic review of protection motivation theory. J Appl
    Soc Psychol. 2000;30(1):106–43.

    42. Grindley EJ, Zizzi SJ, Nasypany AM. Use of protection motivation theory,
    affect, and barriers to understand and predict adherence to outpatient
    rehabilitation. Phys Ther. 2008;88(12):1529–40.

    43. Ministry of Health’ notice on publishing the “proposal for emergency
    response to highly pathogenic avian influenza”[in Chinese]. [http://www.
    gov.cn/zwgk/2006-07/11/content_333087.htm]. Accessed 5 Mar 2017.

    44. Cortina JM. What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and
    applications. J Appl Psychol. 1993;78(1):98–104.

    45. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 3rd ed.
    New York: Guiford; 2011.

    46. Satorra A, Bentler PM. Ensuring Positiveness of the scaled difference
    chi-square test statistic. Psychometrika. 2010;75(2):243–8.

    47. Grewal R, Cote JA, Baumgartner H. Multicollinearity and measurement error
    in structural equation models: implications for theory testing. Mark Sci.
    2004;23(4):519–29.

    48. de Zwart O, Veldhuijzen IK, Richardus JH, Brug J. Monitoring of risk
    perceptions and correlates of precautionary behaviour related to human
    avian influenza during 2006–2007 in the Netherlands: results of seven
    consecutive surveys. BMC Infect Dis. 2010;10:114.

    49. Kuo PC, Huang JH, Liu MD. Avian influenza risk perception and
    preventive behavior among traditional market workers and shoppers in
    Taiwan: practical implications for prevention. PloS One. 2011;6(9):e24157.
    doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0024157.

    50. Ouellette JA, Wood W. Habit and intention in everyday life: the multiple
    processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychol Bull.
    1998;124(1):54–74.

    • We accept pre-submission inquiries
    • Our selector tool helps you to find the most relevant journal
    • We provide round the clock customer support
    • Convenient online submission
    • Thorough peer review
    • Inclusion in PubMed and all major indexing services
    • Maximum visibility for your research

    Submit your manuscript at
    www.biomedcentral.com/submit

    Submit your next manuscript to BioMed Central
    and we will help you at every step:

    Cui et al. BMC Public Health (2017) 17:463 Page 13 of 13

    http://dx.doi.org/10.2471/BLT.13.125989

    http://www.gov.cn/zwgk/2006-07/11/content_333087.htm

    http://www.gov.cn/zwgk/2006-07/11/content_333087.htm

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0024157

      Abstract
      Background
      Methods
      Results
      Conclusion
      Background
      Methods
      The theoretical framework
      Sampling
      Ethics, consent and permissions
      Study instrument
      Data analysis
      Results
      The participants
      A/H7N9 risk perceptions, intention to adopt and actual adoption of PPBs against A/H7N9
      Relationships among demographics, PMT constructs and intention to adopt PPBs
      Information trust and the moderated effects of demographics on the relationships between information trust and PMT constructs
      Discussion
      Conclusions
      Additional files
      Abbreviations
      Acknowledgements
      Availability of data and materials
      Funding
      Authors’ contributions
      Competing interests
      Consent for publication
      Ethics approval and consent to participate
      Publisher’s Note
      Author details
      References

    7

    Analysisof a Professional Journal Article for readability
    Assignment

    Updated 10/13/

    2

    020

    Assignment due: October 20, 2020

    Assignment length: a full 2-3 pages, single spaced, blank line between paragraphs. Maximum

    4

    . No longer than that

    Memorandum format (Markel text, Chapter 15).

    Percentage of course grade: 10%

    Where do we begin?
    The library presentation (discussed in class) is the beginning of the Analysis of a Professional Journal Article for Readability assignment. The engineering librarian discussed the basic services available at the library and showed you the engineering-oriented databases.

    This demo is important for this assignment and can be very helpful in your other engineering classes. During the library demo, you had an opportunity to explore some scholarly journals and possibly pick the one that has the article for your analysis.

    What’s next?

    The assignment asks you to select and analyze a professional journal article from a high quality peer-reviewed journal** on a topic related to your team’s final project. You will determine if:

    It is it a readable technical document, and

    The authors use a valid scientific approach to substantiate their claims.

    You will select a journal with a publication date between 2014-2019.

    Note the differences between professional journals and trade magazines. This makes a difference in trusting the validity of the data. The librarian will also help us with this.

    Our textbook (Markel) will be a helpful resource to your professional journal article analysis for “readability” and “writing qualities”.

    The Martin Luther King Library will also help with understanding scientific methods–real science. Topics include:

    Secondary Research

    Evaluating Print and Online Sources

    Scientific Method

    As you start this assignment:

    Determine the article’s main topic and get a general idea of what it is about and how it is organized. Make a note of what the article’s purpose is and the article’s intended audience.

    Next, look at your technical writing text (Chapters 17, 18, 19) and determine what the author summarizes as the criteria for a good report. Then read the article carefully, and while you are reading it, annotate where the author uses strategies to convey his/her message.

    2

    Consider these questions: Is the article well organized? Is there proper grammar and punctuation? Are there tables, charts, and graphs? Are they helpful? Are other visual aids needed?

    Analyze the introduction, the main discussion, and the terminal section for the validity of the content. Analyze the document to determine if the reasoning is based on pseudoscience, or ‘junk’ science versus real science (examples in lecture and text). Provide examples in your memo. More than likely, articles from professional technical journals apply scientific methodologies. Note how the authors do this.

    Write a formal memo to your instructor with your evaluation.

    You need an introduction

    For clarity, use subject headings

    Do not restate or summarize the contents of the article; focus on the specific analysis and incorporate specific evidence (quotations) to illustrate your points.

    Are the purpose and audience properly addressed? Your memo has two major sections (readability/writing qualities and scientific methodologies). For scientific methodologies:

    a.     Briefly explain the scientific method or the design process used in the article. Markel’s text will help with understanding scientific methods and real science. See Markel’s Experiments showing the scientific method: p. 134. Cunningham’s Principles of Environmental Science will also be helpful.  See section 1.4 “Science helps us understand our world” and section 1.5 “Critical thinking.”

    b.     Briefly explain pseudoscience. Use critical thinking, and check for pseudoscience versus real science (examples in lecture and text). Discuss why this article is not pseudoscience. Provide examples in your report..

    Remember, you need a conclusion/recommendation
    When you have evaluated the report and searched for the strong and weak points, you should be better prepared to write and evaluate your own reports in the future. Would this be a good professional journal for you to submit an article that you have written?

    Give the journal article reference at the bottom of your memo in APA format.* (See also, section in Markel text)

    The memo must be submitted to Canvas.

    *APA Documentation

    WHY APA for documentation and references?

    4

    More engineering schools now use APA rather than the other formats. (However, many professors use

    IEEE

    or MLA.) Always go to your audience and use what they want. Most non-EE advisors now want APA for the following reasons.

    With cut and paste and rearranging articles, you never lose the citation, which can happen when you forget what, for example, #8 or #10 was. You always know when it says (Jones, 2012).

    When you use a numbering system, you have to provide a number in the back for each one. If you use a source 12 times, it will have 12 different numbers in the back. If you use APA, it is just stated once in the back, alphabetically.

    Approximately 94% of the audience never looks in the back for the notes or references. In APA, the audience immediately sees the author and the date. The date is important in engineering and technical writing–to make sure the information is current.

    Sample Journal Article References in APA:

    Journal article (hard copy)

    Linsdell, J., & Anagnos, T. (2011) Motivating technical writing through study of the environment,

    Journal of Professional Issues in Engineering Education and Practice

    , ASCE, 137, 20-27.

    Journal article (viewed online, retrieved electronically, with no DOI assigned. DOI stands for digital object identifier.)

    Srivastava, R. K., & More, A. T. (2010). Some aesthetic considerations for over-the- counter (OTC) pharmaceutical products. International Journal of Biotechnology, 11(3-4), 267-283. Retrieved from http://www.inderscience.com

    Journal article preprint version of article with DOI assigned

    Wang, T. J., Larson M. G., Vasan, R. S., & Gerszten, R.E. (2011). Metabolite profiles and the risk of developing diabetes. Nature Medicine. Advance online publication, doi:10.1038/nm.2307

    **Short List of Examples of Professional Technical Journals (There are over 15,000 professional journals — find one that meets your research needs.

    (Librarian will have more information about this.)

    American Society of Mechanical Engineers

    Journal of Electronic Packaging

    Journal of Fuel Cell Science and Technology

    Journal of Mechanical Design

    Journal of Solar Energy Engineering

    Journal of Computing and Information Science in Engineering

    Journal of Applied Mechanics

    Association of Computer Engineers and Technicians Computer Science and Engineering Society American Society of Civil Engineers

    Journal of Professional Issues in Engineering Education and Practice

    American Institute for Medical and Biological Engineering (AIMBE)

    Journal of Medical Devices

    International Journal of Biotechnology Institute of Industrial Engineers American Society of Safety Engineers

    Association for Computing Machinery (Journal of the ACM) ACM Transactions on Software Engineering Methodology American Institute of Aeronautics and Astronautics

    AIAA Journal

    Journal of Aerospace Computing, Information, and Communication

    Journal of Guidance, Control, and Dynamics

    Journal of Spacecraft and Rock

    American Association for Artificial Intelligence

    Artificial Intelligence Journal

    IEEE

    IEEE Transactions on Circuits and Systems for Video Technology

    IEEE Transactions on Components and Packaging Technologies

    IEEE Transactions on Consumer Electronics

    IEEE Transactions on Electron Devices

    IEEE Transactions on Software Engineering

    IEEE Transactions on Wireless Communications

    IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics

    Society of Manufacturing Engineers (e.g., Journal of Manufacturing Systems)

    Chemical Engineering

    Journal of Chemical Engineering and Materials Science

    Canadian Society for Chemical Engineering (CSChE)

    Journal of Composite Materials

    Reminder:

    Plagiarism will result in a grade of F. Papers with plagiarism cannot be rewritten for credit. You can check for plagiarism with Turnitin.com

    [Simple memo format]

    To: XXXXXX

    From: Student

    Subject: Analysis of a Professional Journal

    Date: XXX, XX, 2011

    [Provide an introduction to the analysis paper

    Use section headings

    Paragraphs are single-spaced with a space in between. Font is 12 point. Min 2 pages, Max 3 pages.]

    Introduction

    [Which article are you evaluating? Where was it published and who are the authors? When was it published?]

    I am evaluating a journal article entitled: The face of success inferences from chief executive officers’ appearance predict company profits. [note only first word capitalized. No italics or quotes.]The article was published in Psychological Science, [note journal title is in italics] in February 2008. [note no comma after February. But if you write February 14, 2008 you need a comma to separate the day 14 from the year 2008.] The authors of the article are Nicholas O. Rule and Nalini Ambady from Tufts University.

    Evaluation [This is the analysis and critique of the journal article]

    The purpose of this article is to present results of a short study done at Tufts University. The study investigated whether or not there is evidence that a company’s success can be predicted based purely on the subjective looks of the CEO. The article is directed mainly at the psychological science community, but also possibly intended to grab the attention of interested investors looking for a way to predict the future success of a candidate investment. [Previous sentence describes audience.]The article is written at an appropriate level for the science community, but there are some places where the meanings of certain statistical variables are not explained. This may not be sufficient for casual readers who may be interested in the article as well. [Provides critique about whether authors achieved the goal of reaching described audience.]

    The article is well organized and the well-labeled sections follow a logical progression. Beginning with a brief introduction that grabs the attention of the reader, the authors also provide background on previous related work. They also explain why the study in the article is unique and important. [Provides critique about introduction, structure of article. The student says there’s background on previous related work. Do you notice if there is a literature review?]

    Next, the article follows with a detailed description of the methods used, explaining how the experiment was set up and why it was designed as it was. It was in this section, however, that several statistical variables were introduced without a definition for what they mean. The science community is familiar with these variables, so for them this is not a problem. However readers without a science background may be left without a good understanding. [Provides critique about methods and difficulty of understanding variables.]

    In terms of grammar, punctuation and style, the authors have done a good job . However, some of the figures presented in the document are not clear. For instance, the formatting on the table of results appears to be confusing and difficult to read. It shows text headings on the vertical axis but only numbers on the horizontal axis. The author has, however, placed the word “Measure” is in the upper left hand corner. This is confusing because some readers may interpret the horizontal numbers to be the measure values that were used in the experiment. This is especially the case, because only four lines before the table, the authors write that the tests were conducted using a seven-point scale, and the numbers on the horizontal axis of the table are labeled one through six. Furthermore, some entries of the table are left blank, and the authors don’t provide the reader with a clear explanation as to why. Finally, in the note below the table, there are more statistical variables that are used without being defined in the article.

    [Provides critique about methods.]The statistical methods applied to the data are scientific, however, the experiment as a whole leaves more work to be done. This is appropriate, because the authors do not claim that their results are definitive, but rather that they have shown some evidence of the conclusions they have. One improvement that could add to the scientific value is increasing the number of companies tested as well as the range of the companies’ successes. Fifty companies were used, but all of them were in the Fortune 1000, so all were highly successful. More scientific conclusions could be made if a full range of companies was tested, including companies that have failed. The experiment may also be more scientifically significant with a wider range of evaluators. The tests were done with only undergraduate college students ranging in age from 18 to 22. Greater statistical significance could be achieved with more subjects over a wider age range. Still, the correlation numbers that were measured were very high in several cases, allowing the authors to justifiably claim significant evidence in their findings.

    [Provides critique about possible pseudoscience .]One aspect of the study that may be considered pseudoscience is the way in which the CEOs’ physical attributes were measured. The measurements were very subjective, essentially the opinions of the test subjects. Furthermore, it may be the case that companies that make more money simply pay for more expensive photographers to take the pictures of the CEOs that will be published online. This would also explain a high correlation between highly rated CEO photos and companies’ financial success. Additionally, these photos may have been taken after the companies were already successful. It would have been a good idea to ensure that all CEO photos were taken before the companies achieved their revenues and profits. This would strengthen the argument that the companies’ success could have been “predicted” by the photos.

    Conclusion

    [Provides overall conclusion and what could be improved.] In all, most of the article is well written and informative for scientific readers as well as readers of casual interest. The experiment was a short study, but the results appear to be statistically significant. The authors claim an appropriate level of significance by stating only that their results show evidence of an effect. Again, the writing style is well organized and grammatically correct. The content is presented in a logical order, but more definition and explanation of variables could help a non-scientific reader. However, since this article was published in a psychological science journal, however, the authors’ objectives were successfully met.

    Only one reference at end of paper, in APA Style. Make sure the style is correct for the journal article you analyzed.

    Reference

    Rule, N. O., & Ambady, N. (2008). The Faces of Success Inferences From Chief Executive Officers’ Appearance Predict Company Profits. Psychological Science, 19(2), 109-111. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02054.x

    Calculate your order
    Pages (275 words)
    Standard price: $0.00
    Client Reviews
    4.9
    Sitejabber
    4.6
    Trustpilot
    4.8
    Our Guarantees
    100% Confidentiality
    Information about customers is confidential and never disclosed to third parties.
    Original Writing
    We complete all papers from scratch. You can get a plagiarism report.
    Timely Delivery
    No missed deadlines – 97% of assignments are completed in time.
    Money Back
    If you're confident that a writer didn't follow your order details, ask for a refund.

    Calculate the price of your order

    You will get a personal manager and a discount.
    We'll send you the first draft for approval by at
    Total price:
    $0.00
    Power up Your Academic Success with the
    Team of Professionals. We’ve Got Your Back.
    Power up Your Study Success with Experts We’ve Got Your Back.

    Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code ESSAYHELP