Literature and Problem Statement

 For this Discussion, you will evaluate the purpose statements in assigned journal articles in your discipline and consider the alignment of theory, problem, and purpose. You will also explain your position on the relationship between research and social change. 

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

 

Alignment means that a research study possesses clear and logical connections among all of its various components. To achieve these connections, researchers must carefully craft the components of their study such that when they are viewed together, there is a coherent interrelationship.

As you read the authors’ purpose statements, consider how well the intent of the study, and its connection to the problem and theoretical framework, is presented. Also, consider if the purpose statement reveals the study’s potential for engendering positive social change.

As you know, social change is a distinguishing feature of Walden University’s mission. Positive social change implies a transformation that results in positive outcomes. This can happen at many levels (e.g., individual, family systems, neighborhoods, organizations, nationally and globally); and positive social change can occur at different rates: slow and gradual or fast and radical.

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

  Journal Article

Wilhelmy, A., Kleinmann, M., König, C. J., Melchers, K. G., & Truxillo, D. M. (2016). How and why do interviewers try to make impressions on applicants? A qualitative study. Journal of Applied Psychology, 101(3), 313-332. doi:10.1037/apl0000046

 

Post a critique of the research study in which you:

  • Evaluate the authors’ use of literature.
  • Evaluate the research problem.
  • Explain what it means for a research study to be justified and grounded in the literature; then, explain what it means for a problem to be original.

The Use of Literature Checklist and Problem Statement Checklist serve as guides for your evaluations. Please do not respond to the checklists in a Yes/No format in writing your Discussion post.

Be sure to support your Main Issue Post and Response Post with reference to the week’s Learning Resources and other scholarly evidence in APA Style.

ResearchTheory, Design, and Methods Walden University

© 2016 Laureate Education, Inc. Page 1 of 1

  • Use of Literature Checklist
  • Use the following criteria to evaluate an author’s use of literature.

    • Look for indications of the following ways the author used literature:

    • Introduce a problem

    • Introduce a theory

    • Provide direction to the research questions and/or hypotheses

    • Compare results with existing literature or predictions

    • Did the author mention the problem addressed by the study?

    • Is the purpose of the study stated?

    • Are key variables in the study defined?

    • Is information about the sample, population, or participants provided?

    • Are the key results of the study summarized?

    • Does the author provide a critique of the literature?

    • Are sources cited to support points?

    • Are the citations to recent literature (within the past 5 years with the exception
    of seminal works)?

    • Does the literature justify the importance of the topic studied?

      Use of Literature Checklist

    Research Theory, Design, and Methods Walden University

    © 2016 Laureate Education, Inc. Page 1 of 1

  • Problem Statement Checklist
  • Use the following criteria to evaluate an author’s problem statement:

    • Is a problem identified that leads to the need for this study?

    • Is a rationale or justification for the problem clearly stated?

    • Is the problem framed in a way that is consistent with the research approach?

    • Does the statement convey how the study will address the problem?

    • Are the citations to literature current (i.e., within the past 5 years with the
    exception of seminal works)?

      Problem Statement Checklist

    Litmus Test for a Doctoral-Level Research Problem

    Background on these “litmus test” questions

    · The distinguishing characteristic of doctoral-level research (as opposed to masters level) is that it must make an original contribution to the field. However, students may struggle to identify what will authentically contribute to their field or discipline.

    · The most critical step in making such a contribution is to first identify a research problem with the 4 doctoral hallmarks below. Identifying a doctoral-level research problem is “necessary, but not sufficient,” to produce doctoral-level capstone.

    REQUIRED DOCTORAL HALLMARKS OF THE RESEARCH PROBLEM

    In Walden’s scholar-practitioner model, a research problem shows promise of contributing meaningfully to the field ONLY if the answer to ALL of the following questions is “yes.”

    Yes

    No

    1. JUSTIFIED?

    Is there evidence that this problem is significant to the professional field?

    There must be relevant statistics (expressing an unjust inequality, financial impact, lost efficiency, etc.), documentable discrepancies (e.g., two models that are difficult to reconcile), and/or other scholarly facts that point to the significance and urgency of the problem. The problem must be an authentic “puzzle” that needs solving, not merely a topic that the researcher finds interesting.

    2. GROUNDED IN THE LITERATURE?

    Can the problem be framed in a way that will enable the researcher to either build upon or counter the previously published findings on the topic?

    For most fields, this involves articulating the problem within the context of a theoretical or conceptual framework. Although there are multiple ways to ground a study in the scientific literature, the essential requirement is that the problem is framed in such a way that the new findings will have implications for the previous findings.

    3. ORIGINAL?

    For research doctorates (Ph.D.):
    Does the problem reflect a meaningful gap in the research literature?
    For the professional doctorates (Ed.D. and D.B.A.):
    Does the problem describe a meaningful gap in practice?

    4. AMENABLE TO SCIENTIFIC STUDY?

    Can a scholarly, systematic method of inquiry be applied to address the problem?
    The framing of the problem should not reveal bias or present a foregone conclusion. Even if the researcher has a strong opinion on the expected findings, scholarly objectivity must be maximized by framing the problem in the context of a systematic inquiry that permits multiple possible conclusions.

    Seediscussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/279955332

    How and Why Do Interviewers Try to Make Impressions on Applicants? A

    Qualitative Study

    Article  in  Journal of Applied Psychology · March 2016

    DOI: 10.1037/apl0000046

    CITATIONS

    44
    READS

    1,131

    5 authors, including:

    Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

    personality testing during personnel selection View project

    Procrastination View project

    Annika Wilhelmy

    University of Zurich

    17 PUBLICATIONS   70 CITATIONS   

    SEE PROFILE

    Martin Kleinmann

    University of Zurich

    115 PUBLICATIONS   1,874 CITATIONS   

    SEE PROFILE

    Cornelius J. König

    Universität des Saarlandes

    168 PUBLICATIONS   3,239 CITATIONS   

    SEE PROFILE

    Klaus Melchers

    Ulm University

    89 PUBLICATIONS   1,448 CITATIONS   

    SEE PROFILE

    All content following this page was uploaded by Annika Wilhelmy on 08 October 2015.

    The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.

    https://www.researchgate.net/publication/279955332_How_and_Why_Do_Interviewers_Try_to_Make_Impressions_on_Applicants_A_Qualitative_Study?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/publication/279955332_How_and_Why_Do_Interviewers_Try_to_Make_Impressions_on_Applicants_A_Qualitative_Study?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/project/personality-testing-during-personnel-selection?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/project/Procrastination-3?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Annika_Wilhelmy?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Annika_Wilhelmy?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_Zurich?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Annika_Wilhelmy?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Martin_Kleinmann?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Martin_Kleinmann?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_Zurich?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Martin_Kleinmann?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Cornelius_Koenig?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Cornelius_Koenig?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Universitaet_des_Saarlandes?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Cornelius_Koenig?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Klaus_Melchers?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Klaus_Melchers?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/institution/Ulm_University?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Klaus_Melchers?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Annika_Wilhelmy?enrichId=rgreq-a6f1f5f5e7a40dfcb6a30ca820afe70b-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzI3OTk1NTMzMjtBUzoyODIyNTAwNDc0NDI5NDRAMTQ0NDMwNTA2MzM5OA%3D%3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 1

    How and Why Do Interviewers Try To Make Impressions on Applicants? A Qualitative Study

    Annika Wilhelmy and Martin Kleinmann
    Universität Zürich

    Cornelius J. König

    Universität des Saarlandes

    Klaus G. Melchers
    Universität Ulm

    Donald M. Truxillo

    Portland State University

    This article is currently in press in Journal of Applied Psychology

    Author Note

    Annika Wilhelmy and Martin Kleinmann, Department of Psychology, Universität Zürich,

    Switzerland; Cornelius J. König, Department of Psychology, Universität des Saarlandes, Germany;

    Klaus G. Melchers, Institute of Psychology and Education, Universität Ulm, Germany; Donald M.

    Truxillo, Department of Psychology, Portland State University, Oregon, USA.

    We thank Talya N. Bauer and Adrian Bangerter for their helpful comments on earlier versions

    of the paper. We are grateful to Stéphanie Weissert, Lisa Juliane Schneider, Romana Nussbaumer,

    and Sabrina Engeli for their help with data collection and analysis, and to Michel Hunziker for his

    help with data analysis. We would also like to thank Susanne Inglin, Domenico Amendola, and

    Roger Keller for technical and methodological consultations.

    Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Annika Wilhelmy, Department

    of Psychology, Universität Zürich, Binzmuehlestrasse 14/12, 8050 Zurich, Switzerland. E-mail:

    a.wilhelmy@psychologie.uzh.ch.

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 2

    Abstract

    To remain viable in today’s highly competitive business environments, it is crucial for organizations

    to attract and retain top candidates. Hence, interviewers have the goal not only of identifying

    promising applicants, but also of representing their organization. Although it has been proposed that

    interviewers’ deliberate signaling behaviors are a key factor for attracting applicants and thus for

    ensuring organizations’ success, no conceptual model about impression management (IM) exists

    from the viewpoint of the interviewer as separate from the applicant. To develop such a conceptual

    model on how and why interviewers use IM, our qualitative

    study

    elaborates signaling theory in the

    interview context by identifying the broad range of impressions that interviewers intend to create for

    applicants, what kinds of signals interviewers deliberately use to create their intended impressions,

    and what outcomes they pursue. Following a grounded theory approach, multiple raters analyzed in-

    depth interviews with interviewers and applicants. We also observed actual employment interviews

    and analyzed memos and image brochures to generate a conceptual model of interviewer IM.

    Results

    showed that the spectrum of interviewers’ IM intentions goes well beyond what has been proposed in

    past research. Furthermore, interviewers apply a broad range of IM behaviors, including verbal and

    nonverbal as well as paraverbal, artifactual, and administrative behaviors. An extensive taxonomy of

    interviewer IM intentions, behaviors, and intended outcomes is developed, interrelationships between

    these elements are presented, and avenues for future research are derived.

    Keywords: employment interview, impression management, signaling theory, recruitment, qualitative

    study

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 3

    How and Why Do Interviewers Try to Make Impressions on Applicants?

    A Qualitative Study

    The employment interview continues to be the most popular selection tool used by both

    applicants and organizations to assess and select one another (Macan, 2009). It is characterized by

    social exchange processes between applicants (who want to get hired) and representatives of the

    organization (who want to attract and select the best candidates). To reach their goals, applicants and

    interviewers try to detect what their interaction partner is interested in and try to use this information

    to send appropriate signals (Bangerter, Roulin, & König, 2012).

    Signaling processes in the interview have mainly been studied in terms of impression

    management (IM) efforts (Delery & Kacmar, 1998). Scholars have repeatedly pointed out that

    interviewers frequently use IM, and that these deliberate behaviors are a key factor for attracting

    applicants and thus ensuring an organization’s economic success (e.g., Dipboye & Johnson, 2013;

    Rosenfeld, 1997). However, it is striking that past interview research has rarely addressed the

    phenomenon of interviewer IM, as most prior studies have limited their focus on how applicants use

    IM (Koslowsky & Pindek, 2011). Furthermore, research has assumed that interviewers use the same

    IM behaviors as applicants (e.g., Stevens, Mitchell, & Tripp, 1990) without taking a closer look at

    what interviewers actually do when they interact with

    applicants.

    We define interviewer IM as interviewers’ deliberate attempts to create impressions on

    applicants (cf. Schlenker, 1980) and argue that it is important to identify and explain interviewer

    IM.

    As outlined below, we argue that interviewers’ aims and opportunities may be different from those of

    applicants, and therefore their IM efforts should be somewhat different as well. Furthermore, scholars

    have noted that signaling theory, which is most often used to explain recruitment phenomena

    (Bangerter et al., 2012; Spence, 1973), is currently not well-defined and understood in the context of

    interviewers’ IM intentions and behaviors (Celani & Singh, 2011). Thus, to provide a more

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 4

    comprehensive theoretical understanding of how and why interviewers try to create impressions on

    applicants, it is crucial to learn more about interviewers’ deliberate signaling behaviors as well as

    their underlying intentions.

    Therefore, the aim of the present study is to use a qualitative approach to create a taxonomy

    and a conceptual model by identifying and analyzing the broad range of possible interviewer IM

    intentions, behaviors, and intended outcomes. We use this conceptual model to point out propositions

    for future research on interviewer IM. Drawing on interdependence theory (Rusbult & Van Lange,

    2003), this study sheds light on how interviewer and applicant IM are similar and distinct.

    Furthermore, our study elaborates signaling theory (Bangerter et al., 2012; Spence, 1973) in the

    interview context by gaining insights into specific signals that are deliberately used by interviewers,

    and why these signals are being sent.

    Theoretical Background

    Signaling Processes in the Interview

    The employment interview is a dynamic exchange in which interviewers and applicants

    engage in social interaction, gather information, and create and form impressions (Levashina,

    Hartwell, Morgeson, & Campion, 2014). Consequently, in the last two decades, researchers have

    increasingly considered both interviewer and applicant perspectives and have given more attention to

    how applicants and interviewers intentionally adapt their behaviors to pursue their interests (Dipboye,

    Macan, & Shahani-Denning, 2012).

    In employment interviews, applicants have information that is of interest to interviewers but

    to which interviewers do not necessarily have access (e.g., information about applicants’ personality).

    Similarly, interviewers have information that is of interest to applicants but to which applicants do

    not necessarily have access (e.g., selection criteria). In situations like this, when two parties have

    access to dissimilar information, signaling theory (Bangerter et al., 2012; Spence, 1973) is helpful for

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 5

    describing and explaining behavior. According to this theory, signaling processes consist of several

    elements, such as two primary actors – the signaler, sender, or insider (e.g., the interviewer), and the

    receiver or outsider (e.g., the applicant) – as well as the actual signals sent by the signaler to the

    receiver (Connelly, Certo, Ireland, & Reutzel, 2011). As Connelly et al. (2011) pointed out, the

    signaler can also take an active part in this signaling process. For instance, interviewers can

    deliberately choose whether and how to reduce information asymmetry by intentionally

    communicating (or signaling) certain qualities to applicants who lack this information (Connelly et

    al., 2011).

    In this vein, IM behaviors reflect an intentional way of sending signals (cf. Schlenker, 1980).

    While interviewers’ signals could be anything that is interpreted as a signal by the applicant,

    interviewer IM refers to signals that are deliberately sent by the interviewer. In other words,

    interviewer IM relates to a deliberate facet of signaling theory (Bangerter et al., 2012). In addition, it

    is important to note that any behavior that an interviewer applies could constitute interviewer IM

    behavior if this behavior is shown with the intention to create impressions on applicants (e.g., asking

    challenging interview questions not only because they are part of the interview guide but also with

    the intention to signal the organization’s high performance expectations). Conversely, if an

    interviewer’s behavior is not linked with such an intention (e.g., asking challenging interview

    questions only because they are part of the interview guide), it does not constitute interviewer IM.

    Although signaling theory is the framework most often used to explain recruitment

    phenomena, it is currently not well-defined and understood when it comes to organizational

    representatives’ intentions and deliberate signaling behaviors (Celani & Singh, 2011). To further

    develop signaling theory, there have been calls to view and study signals within their social context,

    such as the context of employment interviews. As such, a typology of signals that are sent in certain

    contexts – like the employment interview – would be of high value to partition these signals into

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 6

    meaningful categories and thus further understand the signaling phenomenon. In addition, research

    would benefit from investigating the incentives of signalers, such as the outcomes they want to

    achieve by using signals (Connelly et al., 2011). Thus, the main focus of this study is on signaling

    intentions, the signals that interviewers deliberately send through their behavior to create applicant

    impressions, and the outcomes interviewers want to achieve.

    Potential signaling on the side of the interviewer. When organizations try to attract and

    retain promising applicants, deliberate signals such as interviewer IM behavior have been proposed

    to be particularly important (Celani & Singh, 2011). Nevertheless, despite extensive calls in the

    literature to examine how and why interviewers intend to affect applicant impressions (cf., Delery &

    Kacmar, 1998; Dipboye & Johnson, 2013; Gilmore, Stevens, Harrel-Cook, & Ferris, 1999; Macan,

    2009), there have been no systematic attempts to examine the broad range of IM behaviors used by

    interviewers. However, evidence suggests that interviewers pursue specific goals and that there are

    certain interviewer characteristics that positively influence applicant attraction (Chapman, Uggerslev,

    Carroll, Piasentin, & Jones, 2005; Derous, 2007).

    It is important to note that only vague categories of behavior have been examined with regard

    to applicants’ perceptions of interviewer behaviors, (e.g., competent behavior, professional behavior,

    friendly behavior, cf. Chapman et al., 2005). Whereas it has been found that certain interviewer

    behaviors and characteristics influence recruiting outcomes, such as perceived interviewer

    personableness, competence, informativeness, trustworthiness, warmth, humor, and job knowledge

    (Carless & Imber, 2007; Chapman et al., 2005), the signals that interviewers deliberately send

    through their behavior to create these intended impressions have not been identified. Knowing more

    about these specific, deliberate signals is crucial because it would help interviewers to influence

    applicant impressions and thus to enhance recruitment success.

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 7

    Furthermore, we do not know to what degree these interviewer behaviors represent IM in

    terms of intentional, goal-directed behaviors. For instance, Tullar (1989) examined on-campus

    interviewer utterances and found that about two-thirds of the utterances could be categorized as being

    structuring (e.g., expanding on a previous statement) and nearly one-third as demonstrating

    equivalence such as mutual identification (e.g., “That is interesting”). Nevertheless, it remains

    unclear whether, how, and why interviewers intentionally adjust their behaviors to create images on

    applicants’ minds, for example, images of being competent, professional, or friendly.

    Potential differences between applicants’ and interviewers’ signaling. Applicants and

    interviewers find themselves in the same social setting, but it might be misleading to apply existing

    applicant IM taxonomies to interviewers. There may be considerable differences in applicants’ and

    interviewers’ roles, intentions, and scopes of action. Interdependence theory (Rusbult & Van Lange,

    2003) focuses on the causal determinants of dyadic social behavior and provides a conceptual

    framework on the structure of interpersonal situations. The main idea of this theory is that

    characteristics of the situation (e.g., individuals’ interests, information, and level of dependence)

    exert strong effects on individuals’ behavior, for example, IM behavior. Thus, although interviewers

    should apply some IM behaviors similar to those of applicants, they should also apply different IM

    behaviors because they differ from applicants regarding several situational characteristics.

    First, interdependence theory (Rusbult & Van Lange, 2003) suggests that individuals are

    likely to use IM in different ways when they pursue different goals. As pointed out by Bangerter et

    al. (2012), applicants and interviewers have partly divergent interests. For instance, while applicants’

    primary signaling interest is to get a job offer, one of interviewers’ interests is to identify, attract and

    finally hire the best performer. With this end in mind, interviewers try to create an image not only of

    themselves but also of the job and the organization as a whole (Connelly et al., 2011). In other words,

    interviewers need to influence applicants’ image of multiple targets. Thus, in addition to IM

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 8

    behaviors that we know from applicant IM research such as self-promotion or self-focused IM

    behaviors (i.e., describing one’s past accomplishments and competencies in a positive way), and

    ingratiation or other-focused IM behaviors (i.e., flattering one’s interaction partner), interviewers

    may use additional strategies to promote the job and the organization.

    Furthermore, many existing taxonomies distinguish between assertive IM behaviors that aim

    to enhance one’s own image and defensive IM behaviors that aim at defending against threats to a

    positive image (e.g., Ellis, West, Ryan, & DeShon, 2002; Van Iddekinge, McFarland, & Raymark,

    2007). However, in addition to the goal of promoting or defending oneself, the job, and the

    organization, interviewers have also been given recommendations to provide realistic information to

    facilitate self-selection (Wanous, 1976) and to signal honesty (Earnest, Allen, & Landis, 2011). Thus,

    in order to create realistic applicant impressions, interviewers may apply behaviors that go beyond

    applicant IM and that should result in a broader range of IM behaviors than the ones that applicants

    apply.

    Second, according to interdependence theory (Rusbult & Van Lange, 2003), individuals’

    behavior is influenced by the information that is available to them. This is particularly relevant in

    employment interviews, which involve interaction between strangers and are characterized by the

    presence of vague information about the other (Rusbult & Van Lange, 2003). For example,

    interviewers have access to information on applicants’ past failures, potential weaknesses, and gaps

    in the applicants’ CV – whereas applicants usually do not easily get information before the interview

    regarding the job, the organization, and the interviewer. This depth of interviewers’ information on

    applicants should give them more possibilities to deliberately send signals and should thus translate

    into a broader set of IM behaviors as compared to applicants.

    For example, while research on applicant IM has primarily focused on verbal IM behaviors

    (i.e., the content of applicants’ responses and statements), scholars have pointed out that much more

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 9

    could be considered as part of one’s attempt to create images (Dipboye et al., 2012). For instance,

    nonverbal IM has been seen as a fruitful area of research, including IM behaviors such as smiling,

    eye contact, body posture (Levine & Feldman, 2002), as well as head nods, handshakes, and hand

    gestures (McFarland, Yun, Harold, Viera, & Moore, 2005). In addition, verbal behaviors through

    ways other than words may be used, also referred to as paraverbal or paralinguistic behaviors

    (DeGroot & Motowidlo, 1999). Examples of paraverbal behaviors include style of delivery (e.g.,

    pitch and speech rate) and verbal fluency.

    Third, interviewers and applicants are to some extent dependent upon each other, but in

    distinct ways, which should result in some differences in their IM (Rusbult & Van Lange, 2003). For

    instance, applicants rely on interviewers because interviewers’ evaluations affect their chances of a

    job offer (cf. Barrick, Shaffer, & DeGrassi, 2009). Therefore, applicants aim to create positive

    images. Similarly, interviewers depend on applicants in terms of applicants’ job choice behavior, and

    hence intend to create impressions on applicants (Dipboye et al., 2012). However, interviewers are

    usually in a more powerful position than applicants because applicants only get to make a decision

    about whether or not to work for the organization if they are offered a job (Anderson, 1992).

    Consequently, interviewers might have the intention of signaling this power by using IM behaviors

    that go beyond applicants’ IM.

    Aims of the Present Study

    In summary, interviewers’ goals and opportunities for IM are likely to differ from applicants’

    goals and opportunities. Therefore, to enhance our theoretical understanding of this phenomenon, it is

    crucial to develop a comprehensive taxonomy and a conceptual model about the deliberate signaling

    processes on the side of the interviewer in terms of interviewer IM. To address these empirical and

    theoretical gaps, we want to explore three main questions with our qualitative study. Based on these

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 10

    research questions, our aim is to develop a conceptual model and a taxonomy about how and why

    interviewers apply IM.

    Research Question 1: What do interviewers intend to signal to applicants, that is, what

    are interviewers’ IM intentions?

    Research Question 2: What signals do interviewers deliberately use to create their

    intended impressions, that is, what IM behaviors do interviewers apply?

    Research Question 3: What outcomes do interviewers want to achieve by deliberately

    sending signals to applicants, that is, what are interviewers’ intended IM outcomes?

    Method

    Grounded Theory Approach

    Grounded theory is a qualitative methodology that is particularly appropriate for our study

    because it has been developed to understand phenomena about which little is known (Glaser &

    Strauss, 1967) – such as interviewer IM. In addition, grounded theory has been shown to help

    researchers understand complex social processes (Willig, 2009). Thus, it has been suggested that

    researchers apply qualitative research strategies, like grounded theory, in employment interview and

    IM research (cf. Macan,

    2009).

    A core characteristic of grounded theory research is that data collection and analysis are

    closely interrelated to engage with a phenomenon as deeply as possible. As such, analyzing data

    influences the strategy of data collection and vice-versa (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Hence, in our

    study, data analysis influenced our subsequent choice of participants, interview questions,

    observation emphasis, and our choice of topics for further data analysis.

    Furthermore, grounded theory involves collecting data from multiple sources using multiple

    techniques and analyzing it from multiple perspectives to create a multi-faceted sense of the

    phenomenon (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Thus, following recommendations by Bluhm, Herman, Lee,

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 11

    and Mitchell (2011), we sampled diverse interviewers and applicants and collected comprehensive

    information from in-depth interviews with interviewers and applicants, observations of selection

    interviews, the review of memos related to these in-depth interviews and observations, and the

    review of informational material that was given or recommended to applicants during the interview.

    These data were analyzed and discussed by multiple researchers (following recommendations by

    Corbin & Strauss, 2008).

    Moreover, according to grounded theory, data collection and analysis continues until no new

    information is gained, that is, until no new categories and concepts emerge from the data. In the

    present study, this point, which is called theoretical saturation (Glaser & Strauss, 1967), was reached

    after analyzing 30 in-depth interviews, 10 observations of real employment interviews, 43 memos,

    and 12 pieces of informational material.

    Samples

    To better understand interviewers’ IM behaviors, we studied samples of populations who had

    firsthand experience with the social interaction processes in employment interviews: people regularly

    conducting employment interviews (i.e., interviewers) and people who had recently been interviewed

    in several employment interviews (i.e., applicants). We included applicants because signalers (i.e.,

    interviewers) might not always report all of the signals they apply. Specifically, we used information

    provided by applicants to develop ideas about possible interviewer IM intentions and behaviors. We

    then asked interviewers whether the behaviors and intentions reported by applicants actually

    represented deliberate interviewer IM.

    To achieve high heterogeneity of data sources, we began our study with different variables in

    mind that might influence interviewer IM, such as gender, age, interview experience, hierarchical

    level, and educational level (Dipboye, 2005). Interviewers were 27 to 63 years old (M = 41.5, SD =

    12.2), and 60.0% were male. Their interview experience ranged from several months to 40 years, and

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 12

    the number of interviews conducted in the past 12 months ranged from 4 to 300. Furthermore, their

    hierarchical levels were very diverse ranging from assistant positions (e.g., HR assistant) to senior

    manager positions (e.g., commanding officer in the army), and their vacancies ranged from trainee

    and administrative positions to positions with managerial functions. The industry sectors of these

    vacancies were also very diverse, such as human health services, financial services, and the army.

    Applicants were 25 to 46 years old (M = 31.1, SD = 7.7), and 33.3% were male. Their

    interview experience was very diverse, ranging from 5 to 30 interviews, and the number of interviews

    in which they had participated in the past 12 months ranged from 3 to 11. Furthermore, our applicant

    sample consisted of people applying for various positions such as paid internships, administrative

    jobs, PhD programs, executive officer, senior consultant, and senior manager positions in various

    industry sectors ranging from human health services, financial services, travel services, to research

    and education.

    Following an approach within grounded theory called theoretical sampling (Eisenhardt &

    Graebner, 2007), we did not determine a priori what kind of and how much data we wanted to

    collect. Instead, we used information gathered during the research process to develop ideas about

    who could be interviewed and observed next. These new data were used to see whether additional

    relevant categories might emerge, whether categories were well established, and whether

    relationships between categories were fully developed (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Thus, later in the

    process, we also approached interviewers and applicants from industry sectors that were not yet

    included in our sample (e.g., manufacturing and gambling services) because industry sectors were

    mentioned as a potentially important aspect by participants. In addition, we purposely included

    organizations that were facing difficulties regarding their reputation (e.g., a wholesale trade service

    organization that had recently faced a scandal) because participants pointed out that this might help to

    capture potential defensive strategies by interviewers. Furthermore, participants’ comments led us

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 13

    additionally to include third-party interviewers (e.g., recruiting consultants) and interviewers within

    an employing organization; interviewers with experience in college recruiting and in initial screening

    interviews in addition to late-stage interviews; and interviewers and applicants with experience in

    telephone interviews, video interviews, and panel interviews (because of the commonness of such

    interviews). Sampling was done through job websites, an alumni pool of a Swiss university, and

    references from our participants.

    Data Collection

    For data collection, we applied several methods as suggested by Bluhm et al. (2011): semi-

    structured in-depth interviews of interviewers and applicants, observations of real employment

    interviews, memos, and review of informational materials provided to applicants. It is important to

    note that behaviors that were observed and ones that were reported by applicants provided us with

    additional ideas of potential IM behaviors that we could verify in subsequent in-depth interviews to

    ensure that these behaviors constituted IM (i.e., that they were applied by interviewers with the

    intention of creating impressions on applicants). The in-depth interviews and observations are further

    described below. Memos (one to two pages) were written subsequent to each in-depth interview and

    observation and during the coding process. They were used to document ideas for data interpretation

    and to engage in self-reflection about potential personal biases (see Glaser & Strauss, 1967; Suddaby,

    2006). Furthermore, as suggested by Bansal and Corley (2011), informational material (such as

    brochures) that was given or recommended to applicants was analyzed.

    In-depth interviews. All of the 30 in-depth interviews (1 hour) with interviewers and

    applicants were conducted by the first author in Switzerland and Germany. Regarding in-depth

    interviews with applicants, the main goal was to develop ideas about what IM intentions interviewers

    might have had and what signals they might have applied to create favorable impressions. Regarding

    the in-depth interviews with interviewers, however, we placed special emphasis on whether they

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 14

    really reported having had these intentions and whether they deliberately engaged in them in terms of

    IM.

    Following an orienting theoretical perspective (Locke, 2001), in-depth interviews were based

    on semi-structured interview guides derived from insights gained during the review of the existing

    literature. As can be seen in Appendices A and B, these interview guides covered four aspects: (a)

    whether the particular impressions that applicants form during interviews might be important to

    interviewers, (b) impressions that interviewers want applicants to form, (c) behaviors that

    interviewers apply to create these favorable impressions, and (d) possible consequences of

    interviewer IM. Part (a) of the interview guide ensured that our participants were concerned about the

    impressions applicants form during the interview. It also prepared the mind-set of our participants

    and stimulated them to take a recruitment perspective on the interview to ensure that we had a

    common basis for the data from all interviews.

    Furthermore, our interview questions were continuously adapted during the data collection

    process depending on the insights we gained (Glaser & Strauss, 1967): Questions asked earlier in the

    research process were different from those asked later as we better understood the interviewers’ and

    applicants’ experiences and contexts (see Appendices A and B). For instance, to verify ideas that

    emerged from applicants’ statements or from observations, we adapted the questions for our in-depth

    interviews with interviewers to verify that these behaviors were intentionally applied IM behaviors

    rather than some other, naturally occurring behavior. Hence, our in-depth interviews became

    increasingly focused over the course of the study.

    At the beginning of each in-depth interview, participants were ensured of confidentiality and

    anonymity during further data processing. They were instructed to answer our questions based on the

    employment interviews they had conducted (or participated in as an applicant) within the past 12

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 15

    months. At the end of each in-depth interview, participants were given a survey that covered

    demographic and context information. Furthermore, we audio-recorded all in-depth interviews.

    Observations. As interviewer IM behaviors might not always be recognized by either

    interviewers or applicants, we decided to observe 10 actual employment interviews. Following

    recommendations by Bluhm et al. (2011), these observations served as an additional data source to

    develop ideas on possible interview IM categories that could be verified in subsequent in-depth

    interviews with interviewers. 

    The observed employment interviews were between 25 minutes and 2 hours long and took

    place in seven different organizations. Two of these employment interviews were with the same

    interviewers. Furthermore, one interviewer took part in both the in-depth interviews and the

    observations. In addition, three of the employment interviews were not only observed but also audio

    or video recorded. To avoid observer-expectancy effects, observation participants were not told that

    this study examined interviewer IM behavior (Kazdin, 1977). Instead, they were briefly informed that

    we were interested in the social processes taking place in employment interviews and were ensured

    confidentiality.

    The first author and a trained I/O Master’s level student conducted all of the observations

    using an observation guide (see Appendix C). The goal of this observation guide was to help consider

    all important aspects of the interview. The guide consisted of three main parts: observations prior to

    the employment interview (e.g., what interviewers say and ask prior to the interview), different kinds

    of interviewers’ IM behavior during the employment interview (e.g., how interviewers talk to the

    applicants during the interview), and observations after the employment interview (e.g., body

    language of interviewers after the interview). In addition, the observation guide contained sections

    for unstructured observations in order to include data that might lead to new interpretations or

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 16

    themes. Similar to the in-depth interview questions, the content of the observation guide was

    constantly adapted in the course of the research process.

    During and after each observation, the observers wrote down which IM behaviors

    interviewers showed on the basis of the observation guide, and noted verbatim what the interviewers

    said. Observed behaviors were described with as much detail as possible. At the end of each

    observation, the observed interviewers filled out a survey that covered demographic and context

    information. As described above, the observed behaviors were then incorporated into the in-depth

    interviews with interviewers to ensure that they actually constituted instances of IM rather than some

    other kind of behavior.

    Data Analysis

    Content analysis. Following grounded theory principles (Glaser & Strauss, 1967; Suddaby,

    2006), all data were analyzed in four main steps. First, data were inspected sentence by sentence by

    two independent raters of a pool of five raters (the first author, the I/O Master’s level student who

    also served as an observer, and three other I/O Master’s level students). Raters participated in a half-

    day training session conducted by the first author to learn and practice how to code (e.g., how to

    apply and modify categories) using the coding software ATLAS.ti 6 (Friese, 2011). The use of two

    coders ensured multiple perspectives on the data, as suggested by Corbin and Strauss (2008) to

    increase creativity in the analysis while also decreasing personal bias. Furthermore, to increase

    immersion in the data content, one of these two coders had always either conducted, observed, or

    transcribed the in-depth interview under investigation and was therefore familiar with the interview

    content. Regarding the coding of the in-depth interview data, interviews were transcribed verbatim

    until we came closer to saturation (i.e., when the number of new categories was decreasing notably).

    This was the case when twenty of the interviews had been transcribed, which totaled 613 double-

    spaced pages. For the remaining ten interviews, tape recordings were directly coded. Observations

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 17

    were coded based on observation notes and, if available, based on audio and video recordings.

    Following Kreiner, Hollensbe, and Sheep (2009), coding was done based on an evolving system of

    categories, a so-called coding dictionary that was continually modified based on iterative

    comparisons between newly coded and previously analyzed data. Each word, sentence, paragraph,

    and passage was seen as a feasible coding unit and could be coded. The ATLAS.ti 6 software was

    used to enter codes, perform text and audio searches, and identify intersections of codes (following

    recommendations by Grzywacz et al., 2007).

    In a second step, the two coders met in joint coding meetings. They compared individual

    codings and discussed discrepancies until consensus was established about which code was

    appropriate. Furthermore, the technique of triangulation was used, meaning that agreement and

    discrepancies among different data sources and different types of data were examined and discussed

    to see whether they led to the same categories (Willig, 2009). For example, our observations of actual

    employment interviews provided particularly valuable insights into nonverbal and artifactual

    interviewer IM behaviors that were not spontaneously reported by interviewers. These behaviors

    were either confirmed when we directly asked interviewers about it (e.g., displaying application

    documents on the interview table), or not confirmed and thus not integrated into our system of

    categories (e.g., displaying one’s security pass).

    In a third step, coders identified abstract categories or concepts at the end of each joint

    meeting to enhance the conceptual structure of the categories. The aim was to ‘lift’ the data to a

    conceptual level by comparing codes and ideas emerging from the data (Martin & Turner, 1986).

    Coders remained attentive to how these abstract concepts were related to existing research, and how

    existing research could be used to identify and name new categories (Locke, 2001). After these

    meetings, any new categories (including descriptions and example quotes), and any category changes

    were documented in the coding dictionary.

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 18

    In a fourth step, to move further from a descriptive to a conceptual level, our analysis focused

    mainly on how the categories were linked (Schilling, 2006), especially on links between interviewer

    IM intentions, behaviors, and intended outcomes. For this purpose, one rater went back to the

    transcripts and audio recordings of the in-depth interviews with interviewers to examine which

    categories were reported together in terms of forming a common theme over the course of each in-

    depth interview. All of the links that were identified were documented to gain an overview about

    which categories were associated, which associations were the strongest, and which patterns of

    associations emerged.

    Interrater agreement. Given the emergent nature of our categories, it was not possible to

    determine interrater agreement during the primary coding process described above. Therefore, we

    engaged in a secondary coding process to test the reliability of our categories and to determine the fit

    of the emergent categories with the data (Butterfield, Trevino, & Ball, 1996). Following Kreiner et al.

    (2009), we gave two of the five coders mentioned above a final version of the coding dictionary that

    had emerged as well as a representative transcript subsample of 60 pages (10 percent, following

    Bluhm et al., 2011) containing 185 interview passages. The coders were instructed to assign each

    interview passage to the category that they believed best represented the passage. The overall

    percentage of agreement between the two coders was .91, and Cohen’s Kappa was .88, suggesting

    very good agreement (Fleiss & Cohen, 1973).

    Member checks. Finally, we conducted member checks (also known as participant checks,

    informant feedback, communicative validation, or respondent validation) to give voice to our

    participants (Bluhm et al., 2011) and to ensure that the categories derived in this study were indeed

    grounded in the data (Yanow & Schwartz-Shea, 2006). Member checks imply that categories are

    tested with members of those stakeholding groups from whom the data were originally collected

    (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). We went back to the 30 participants in the in-depth interviews and asked for

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 19

    their feedback on our categories. Three of these member checks were conducted by telephone, and 23

    were conducted online (an 86.7 % overall response rate).

    First, participants were introduced to all of the categories derived in this study. As we were

    interested in in-depth feedback, participants were then allocated to one of four different groups. Each

    group was given a different subsample of categories to focus on. Regarding this subsample of

    categories, participants were asked to what extent they believed each single category was useful for

    conceptualizing interviewer IM. Specifically, they were asked to indicate whether the behaviors

    represented deliberate interviewer IM in terms of behaviors that are applied to create favorable

    applicant impressions. Second, we asked participants whether any categories should be merged,

    deleted, divided, or added, and whether they would change the categories’ structure. Third, we

    analyzed participants’ ideas and commentaries, went back to our data for confirmation, and

    integrated the results into our system of categories.

    Results

    Overview

    The aim of this study was to investigate how interviewers try to create impressions on

    applicants in terms of interviewer IM intentions and behaviors, and why they engage in these

    behaviors in terms of intended IM outcomes. Regarding interviewer IM intentions, the data analysis

    yielded five categories that we organized into two major themes (see Table 1): primary interviewer

    IM intentions that refer to interviewers’ overriding goal of representing the organization, the job, and

    themselves (i.e., signaling attractiveness and signaling authenticity), and secondary interviewer IM

    intentions that refer to interviewers’ actual personal interactions with applicants (i.e., signaling

    closeness, signaling distance in terms of professionalism, and signaling distance in terms of

    superiority). In terms of interviewer IM behaviors, we found five different types of behavior: verbal,

    paraverbal, nonverbal, artifactual, and administrative interviewer IM behaviors (see Table 2). With

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 20

    regard to intended interviewer IM outcomes, we found three different types: outcomes related to the

    interview’s recruitment function, related to the interview’s selection function, and related to

    interviewers themselves (see Table 3). As can be seen in Tables 1 to 3, these types of IM intentions,

    behaviors, and intended outcomes could each be further differentiated into higher-level (left column)

    and lower-level categories (right column) based on our data. In addition, many of the emergent

    categories were unanticipated by past IM research (indicated by italicized category names in Tables 1

    to 3). Our conceptual model of interviewer IM is depicted in Figure 1 and displays how interviewer

    IM intentions (square boxes), behaviors (round-edged cells), and intended outcomes (at the end of

    arrows outside of boxes) are linked. Please note that the IM behaviors presented in Figure 1 are not

    comprehensive, but constitute representative examples to demonstrate the main patterns of

    relationships that we found between IM intentions, behaviors, and intended outcomes gleaned from

    Tables 1 to 3.

    How Interviewers Apply IM

    What are interviewers’ IM intentions? To gain insights on how interviewers apply IM, we

    analyzed interviewers’ underlying intentions. We found a broad spectrum of impressions that

    interviewers intend to create on applicants, and that different aims or foci can be distinguished. We

    found that interviewers try to influence applicant impressions not only regarding impressions of the

    interviewers themselves, but also regarding impressions of the team, the job, and the organization as

    a whole. For example, one interviewer said1 “The impression I create on the applicant concerning

    myself as a person and concerning our company and our way of working, I think that’s the basis for

    the whole [hiring] process that may start afterwards … What counts is the perception that the

    1 For the sake of brevity, quotes supporting these categories are not presented for all categories but are available from the
    first author upon request.

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 21

    candidate gets of me and everything I’m representing” (Interviewer 62). Hence, compared to

    applicant IM, interviewer IM may be considered as a more complex phenomenon because applicants’

    major (and maybe only) aim is to enhance interviewer impressions of themselves (cf. Barrick et al.,

    2009).

    Furthermore, we noticed that early in the in-depth interviews, participants mainly told us

    about the impressions applicants should receive regarding the organization, the job, and the

    interviewer as a person. As these IM intentions have to do with the main goal of the interviewer (i.e.,

    representing the company) and constitute very basic intentions, we called them “primary” (see Table

    1). Data analysis suggested that interviewer IM serves two main purposes: signaling attractiveness

    (IM intention 1) and signaling authenticity (IM intention 2).

    While the intention of appearing attractive is in line with the dominant understanding of IM

    (e.g., Jones & Pittman, 1982), the intention of appearing authentic adds an important new aspect. It

    suggests that for interviewers, creating realistic images is important not only in terms of realistic job

    previews and self-selection (Wanous, 1976), but also in terms of being perceived as sincere and taken

    seriously by applicants.

    Furthermore, participants told us about additional IM intentions that we called “secondary”

    because in contrast to the two primary IM intentions, these intentions seemed to be more closely

    related to interviewers’ personal interaction with the applicant and were usually mentioned later in

    the in-depth interviews (see Table 1). Regarding secondary interviewer IM intentions, three major

    categories emerged from what interviewers reported in the in-depth interviews: signaling closeness

    (IM intention 3), signaling distance in terms of professionalism (IM intention 4), and signaling

    2 Quotes are labeled with participant code numbers, which either start with “Interviewer” to indicate that an interviewer
    was the source of information or “Applicant” to indicate that an applicant was the source of information. More detailed
    information about any quotes presented in this article is available from the first author upon request.

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 22

    distance in terms of superiority (IM intention 5). As can be seen in Table 1, these secondary

    intentions could each be further differentiated into lower-level categories based on the data.

    Interestingly, the secondary interviewer IM intention of distance in terms of superiority

    indicates that interviewers do not always try to be friendly and build rapport with the applicant. In

    some cases, interviewers might rather have the intention to signal their status and power (IM

    intention 5a) or to convey a feeling of uncertainty to applicants about the likelihood of receiving a job

    offer (IM intention 5c).

    How are interviewers’ IM intentions interrelated? Data analyses revealed various

    interrelations between interviewer IM intentions. For instance, the two primary interviewer IM

    intentions of signaling attractiveness and signaling authenticity were found to constitute two separate

    dimensions that often co-occur with each other (e.g., “It’s not only about a positive impression but

    also about a realistic one,” Interviewer 15). In addition, these two intentions were reported by most

    interviewers, which indicates that signaling attractiveness and signaling authenticity are both

    fundamental for most interviewers.

    Furthermore, our findings show that the secondary IM intentions differ regarding their

    importance for interviewers. Whereas signaling closeness was reported in almost all of the in-depth

    interviews and thus seems to be a universal and fundamental IM intention, signaling distance in terms

    of professionalism and superiority was reported less often and may thus play an important role only

    for some interviewers. Interestingly, those interviewers who reported the intention of signaling

    distance always reported the intention of signaling closeness as well. This provides some indication

    that interviewers can have both intentions simultaneously.

    In addition, we found that all interviewers reported multiple primary and secondary IM

    intentions, and some of these intentions seemed synergetic while others seemed rather incompatible.

    This is also represented in the way the different kinds of impressions are arranged in Figure 1 in

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 23

    terms of being located closer together vs. further apart. For example, interviewers with the intention

    of creating an impression of authenticity often also reported the intention of creating an impression of

    professionalism, such as “There may be companies … that only present the positive and try to

    mislead people, but with us, that’s not the case … I don’t want to persuade [the applicant] of

    something that’s not true. One should be truthful, open, transparent. I don’t think this is about putting

    on a show” (Interviewer 3). In contrast, interviewers with the intention to signal distance in terms of

    superiority rarely reported the intention of signaling attractiveness, indicating that these intentions

    may be rather incompatible for interviewers.

    What IM behaviors do interviewers apply? We found that interviewers apply a broad range

    of different IM behaviors that do not only include verbal and paraverbal behaviors but also

    nonverbal, artifactual, and administrative behaviors (see Table 2).

    Verbal interviewer IM. Verbal interviewer IM means that interviewers use the content of

    what they are saying to influence applicant impressions. As can be seen in Table 2, results suggest

    that verbal interviewer IM behaviors can be divided into self-focused (i.e., interviewer-focused; IM

    behavior 1), applicant-focused (IM behavior 2), fit-focused (IM behavior 3), job-, team-, or

    organization-focused (IM behavior 4), and interview process-focused IM behavior (IM behavior 5).

    Additionally, another form of verbal interviewer IM is modifying one’s style of communication (IM

    behavior 6), such as modifying the applicants’ speech portion, adapting one’s vocabulary and dialect

    to the applicant, and using verbal encouragers (e.g., “mmmh”, “ya”, “yeah”).

    Analysis of our in-depth interviews with interviewers indicated that to place themselves, their

    organization, and the job in a favorable light, interviewers are likely to present positive information

    and express enthusiasm to the applicant (IM behavior 1e). We also found that to induce an

    impression of authenticity, sometimes interviewers intentionally state negative aspects of the

    company or the job such as, “To be authentic and honest, I indicate weaknesses of the company …,

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 24

    indicate the positive but also weaknesses” (Interviewer 10; IM behavior 4c). Furthermore, we found

    that to signal attractiveness despite negative aspects, interviewers often frame negative information in

    a positive way (IM behavior 4d). For example, an interviewer reported, “I personally try to do this in

    a frank way, in a straightforward way … There are negative aspects regarding the work load but, of

    course, that results in a higher quality of our [services]. So negative aspects are justified in a positive

    way” (Interviewer 8).

    Paraverbal interviewer IM. Paraverbal interviewer IM refers to interviewers’ verbal

    behaviors other than words that are applied to influence applicant impressions (cf. Barrick et al.,

    2009; DeGroot & Motowidlo, 1999). As depicted in Table 2, we found three different categories of

    how interviewers modulate their voice when communicating with applicants: speaking in an

    empathetic way to signal closeness (IM behavior 7), speaking in an authoritative way to signal

    distance in terms of superiority (IM behavior 8), and speaking in an unobstrusive, neutral way to

    signal distance in terms of professionalism (IM behavior 9). The finding that interviewers may

    intentionally talk in an authoritative way provides empirical support for propositions by Gilmore et

    al. (1999) and Connerley and Rynes (1997), who suggested that interviewers might sometimes have

    the goal of intimidating applicants.

    Nonverbal interviewer IM. Nonverbal interviewer IM means that interviewers use their body

    language to create impressions on the applicant. As shown in Table 2, we found that interviewers

    may use nonverbal IM both to create an impression of closeness, for example by laughing (IM

    behavior 10a) and making eye contact (IM behavior 10d). For example, an interviewer reported, “To

    make sure it’s casual and comfortable, maybe chuckling with the candidates” (Interviewer 7). In

    addition, data indicated that nonverbal interviewer IM can also be applied in the form of body

    contact. This includes not only handshakes (IM behavior 10i), as suggested by applicant IM research

    (e.g., McFarland et al., 2005), but also friendly backslaps (IM behavior 10j), for example at the end

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 25

    of the interview. Furthermore, we found that interviewers apply elements of empathetic listening (cf.,

    Bodie, 2011) to influence applicant impressions, such as mirroring the applicant’s posture (IM

    behavior 10g) and nodding affirmatively (IM behavior 10c). Data also revealed that a lack of

    empathetic listening may serve as IM (i.e., doing something unrelated to the conduct of the interview,

    IM behavior 10k). To irritate applicants and convey superiority, some interviewers intentionally

    avoided eye contact, for example, by paging through documents or looking out of the window while

    applicants were talking. For example, an interviewer stated “Putting on a poker face, well, I try to

    restrain myself” (Interviewer 7).

    Artifactual interviewer IM. Artifactual interviewer IM refers to how interviewers use “an

    object made by a person” (Hornby & Wehmeier, 2005, p. 72), such as manipulating professional,

    status, and aesthetic cues to influence applicant impressions (Gardner & Martinko, 1988; Schneider,

    1981). As can be seen in Table 2, we found that interviewers use four different kinds of artifacts to

    create images: aspects of their appearance (IM behavior 12), premises appearance (IM behavior 13),

    visual information displayed during the interview (IM behavior 14), and giveaways or promotional

    items for applicants (IM behavior 15).

    First, consistent with applicant IM taxonomies, interviewers reported that they modify their

    clothing (IM behavior 12a) and accessories (IM behavior 12b) to influence impressions. However, in

    contrast to applicants, interviewers were found to also consider the appearance of the interview

    building (IM behavior 13a), interview room (IM behavior 13b), and the seating arrangement (IM

    behavior 13g) as a very important IM tool. For instance, an interviewer said, “A conference room …

    portraying the department, that certainly has a more positive impact than if one gets the impression

    that it’s a chilly cubbyhole” (Interviewer 7; IM behavior 13b). Additionally, regarding the seating

    arrangement, sitting kitty-corner may aim to create impressions of closeness (e.g., “then he [the

    applicant] certainly doesn’t feel so exposed … not like being before the court,” Interviewer 8), while

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 26

    sitting face-to-face may aim to signal superiority (e.g., “it’s always been face-to-face… a typical

    exam situation,” Applicant 1).

    Second, we found that interviewers provide applicants with visual information during the

    interview to convey images. For instance, interviewers reported that they intentionally display

    applicants’ application documents on the table (IM behavior 14b), sometimes marked in bright

    colors, to create a professional image.

    Finally, an aspect that has not been considered in past research is that interviewers may hand

    out giveaways and promotional items to applicants to influence the impressions they gain, such as

    informational material (IM behavior 15a), promotional gifts (IM behavior 15b), and business cards

    (IM behavior 15c). These items can convey appreciation and help to stick in the applicant’s memory.

    Administrative interviewer IM. While collecting and analyzing data, we noticed that many

    interviewers were telling us about how they time their communication and provide services to

    applicants to influence applicant impressions (see Table 2). We called this type of interviewer IM

    “administrative” because it refers to behaviors connected with organizing the interview. Regarding

    timing of communication (IM behavior 16), our study goes beyond existing work on pre-interview

    communication (cf. Carless & Hetherington, 2011) by showing that interviewers may intentionally

    ensure timeliness in order to create applicant impressions of closeness. For instance, one interviewer

    said, “I think in a way it’s appreciation … So it’s fatal when somebody from the line management is

    late for the interview” (Interviewer 8).

    Concerning administrative interviewer IM by providing services to applicants (IM behaviors

    16 to 19), our data suggest that many aspects of conducting interviews that have only been

    understood as standard elements in previous research can actually constitute interviewer IM if these

    behaviors are applied with the intention of creating impressions on applicants. For example,

    interviewers reported sending confirmations of receipt of application documents to applicants (IM

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 27

    behavior 17a) not only because it was part of the standard procedure, but also because they wanted to

    convey appreciation, which makes it IM behavior.

    In addition, interviewers seem to provide services for IM purposes not only during the

    interview, but also beforehand and subsequently. For instance, to create an impression of closeness

    before the actual interview, interviewers may call and invite applicants personally instead of asking

    somebody else to extend an invitation (IM behavior 17c): “I prefer a personal telephone call. That

    makes a completely different impression than an anonymous e-mail …When I talk to the person by

    telephone, it seems much more significant” (Interviewer 11).

    During the interview, offering drinks (IM behavior 18c), breaks (IM behavior 18d), site visits

    (IM behavior 18f), and refund of travel expenses (IM behavior 18g) can be considered as IM, if these

    service features are intended to serve as signals to the applicant (i.e., signals of professionalism). For

    instance, one interviewer reported, “I offer something to drink. Often they [the applicants] don’t even

    have the courage to pour the water themselves. Then I do that as well” (Interviewer 3). However,

    interviewers may also intentionally choose not to offer certain drinks in order to signal

    professionalism, such as “I don’t serve any coffee … I want to lay emphasis on professionalism

    because to me, a selection interview is not an afternoon coffee party” (Interviewer 10).

    After the interview, interviewers were found to intentionally influence applicant impressions

    by modifying their way of giving feedback to applicants about interview results (e.g., by providing

    feedback by telephone instead of by e-mail, IM behavior 19b). For example, an interviewer told us,

    “Usually I do that orally. Communicating that we decided to choose somebody else, I try to do that

    orally, if possible” (Interviewer 3).

    When do interviewers apply which IM behaviors? Our analyses revealed that interviewers

    apply different IM behaviors depending on their IM intentions. These links between interviewer IM

    intentions and behaviors are depicted in Figure 1; behaviors (white, round-edged cells) are placed

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 28

    within or touching intentions (light or dark gray, square boxes) to indicate association. First, we

    found that most of the IM behaviors are used with a certain purpose, that is, there is a clear link

    between each of these IM behaviors and a single IM intention. In Figure 1, this is visualized by cells

    of behavioral examples that are located within each larger box representing an IM intention. For

    example, expressing enthusiasm (IM behavior 1e) and decorating the interview room (IM behavior

    13c) are often used with the intention to signal attractiveness (IM intention 1), whereas demonstrating

    similarity (IM behavior 3b) is often used with the intention to signal closeness (IM intention 3).

    Second, some IM behaviors are related to multiple IM intentions at the same time. In Figure 1, this is

    visualized by cells of behavioral examples that are bridging the boxes of two different IM intentions.

    For instance, positive framing (IM behavior 4d) may be used to signal both attractiveness (IM

    intention 1) and authenticity (IM intention 2), and the IM behavior of incorporating future colleagues

    (IM behavior 18e) may be used to signal both authenticity (IM intention 2) and superiority (IM

    intention 5). It is noteworthy that we only found one IM behavior, challenging, with the goal of

    creating a professional and superior image. Third, the remaining IM behaviors can be described as

    being multipurpose, that is, they can be related to different IM intentions depending on how they are

    applied. In Figure 1, these multipurpose IM behaviors are located in the center of the figure. For

    example, interviewers can modify applicants’ speech portion (IM behavior 6c) in a way that the

    portion is high to signal appreciation (IM intention 3b), or in a way that the portion is low to signal

    status and power of decision (IM intention 5a).

    As described above, we found that most of the variance regarding underlying IM intentions

    lies within the lower-level categories of IM behaviors. However, we also found some indications

    regarding how the five broad categories of IM behaviors (verbal, paraverbal, nonverbal, artifactual,

    and administrative; see Table 2) might be linked to IM intentions (see Figure 1). For example,

    paraverbal and nonverbal IM behaviors may play a particularly important role for signaling closeness

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 29

    versus signaling distance. Paraverbal and nonverbal IM behaviors represent indirect ways of

    communicating, which seems especially important regarding interviewers’ personal interaction with

    applicants, that is, interviewers’ secondary IM intentions.

    In addition, our data suggest that some interviewer IM behaviors are more prevalent than

    others. Some IM behaviors were reported by almost all interviewers, which indicates that these

    behaviors are rather universally applied and fundamental for interviewers (e.g., challenging,

    modifying interview length, offering drinks). In contrast, some IM behaviors were reported only by a

    few interviewers, which indicates that these behaviors are rather idiosyncratic (e.g., backslapping,

    displaying test results, handing out promotional gifts).

    Furthermore, our analyses revealed that those IM behaviors that are linked to the same IM

    intention are most likely to be applied in combination. For instance, if an interviewer aims to signal

    closeness (IM intention 3), IM behaviors such as demonstrating similarity (IM behavior 3b), referring

    to the applicant by name (IM behavior 2a), and thanking (IM behavior 2f) tend to be combined. In

    contrast, IM behaviors that are related to rather incompatible IM intentions are unlikely to be

    combined.

    Additionally, we found that that the use of IM behaviors might depend on the interviewer’s

    industry sector. For instance, when we compared interviewers from the army with interviewers from

    human health services, we found that those from the army reported more intentions to signal

    authenticity and performance expectations to create a realistic image and enhance applicants’ self-

    selection. This is in line with the army’s tough image and clear command structure. In contrast,

    interviewers from the human health services such as hospitals put more emphasis on signaling

    attractiveness by reinforcing the advantages of the job and their respective hospital (IM behavior 4a),

    and put more emphasis on signaling closeness, for example, by stepping up to the applicant before

    the interview (IM behavior 18a). As hospitals are service providers, these interviewers were also

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 30

    much more concerned about applicants’ future consumer behavior (intended IM outcome 4c) in terms

    of choosing their hospital if they require treatment. Thus, an interviewer’s industry sector is likely to

    influence the specific set of IM intentions, behaviors, and intended outcomes that is applied.

    Furthermore, we found that there are some interviewer IM behaviors that can only be applied

    in certain interview settings such as panel interviews. Specifically, regarding nonverbal IM,

    interviewers were found to intentionally smile at other interviewers (IM behavior 11a), nod in

    response to other interviewers’ questions (IM behavior 11b), and mirror other interviewers’ body

    postures (IM behavior 11c) to induce an impression of harmony and signal positive corporate

    climate. These findings suggest that panel interviews may offer interviewers additional possibilities

    for influencing applicant impressions.

    What is more, interviewers may change their IM intentions and behaviors over the course of

    the interview. For instance regarding paraverbal IM, interviewers and applicants reported that

    interviewers tend to speak in an empathetic way (IM behavior 7) at the beginning and end of

    interviews, and when asking delicate questions. In contrast, interviewers tend to speak in an

    authoritative way (IM behavior 8) when asking challenging questions. For example, interviewers

    reported, “I ask questions rather snappily” (Interviewer 7), and “When I want to hear an answer, then

    I express myself in a very bald way, then I’m not welcoming anymore” (Interviewer 11). This

    suggests that the way in which interviewers apply IM might depend on the timing in the interview

    and on the content of the conversation.

    Why Interviewers Apply IM

    What are interviewers’ intended IM outcomes? To examine why interviewers apply IM,

    we asked interviewers and applicants about their experiences and assumptions on intended IM

    outcomes. As can be seen in Figure 1 and Table 3, our data revealed that interviewers try to influence

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 31

    applicant impressions in order to enhance many different outcomes related to recruitment, selection,

    and to interviewers themselves.

    First, interviewer IM may be applied to improve the interview’s recruitment function such as

    a strong organizational image and reputation on the side of the applicant (intended IM outcome 3c).

    For instance, an interviewer reported, “Ideally, in the end the applicant says ‘They did not hire me,

    but this is a GOOD company.’ That’s the goal” (Interviewer 12). In addition, interviewers reported

    that they applied IM to ensure that applicants leave the interview room feeling good about themselves

    (intended IM outcome 2), and react with positive attitudes (intended IM outcome 3), intentions, and

    behaviors towards the organization (intended IM outcome 4). For instance, an interviewer reported,

    “To give the applicant a positive feeling, even in situations where it’s clear that the candidate is not

    qualified … So that the applicant gets an impression of the company, what we do, what we stand for,

    particularly the positive we stand for, and has a positive attitude towards us” (Interviewer 7).

    Second, we found that interviewers not only apply IM for recruitment purposes but also to

    enhance the interview’s selection purpose. Specifically, we found that interviewers intend to increase

    the amount of personal information applicants reveal during the interview (intended IM outcome 5a)

    and the informative value of this information (intended IM outcome 5b) in terms of applicants being

    honest and explicit. For example, one interviewer reported “When I find something in the CV where

    I have experience myself … when the applicant can tell, aha, this person knows what I’ve

    experienced … then he’s more relaxed, tells me more, and is more open towards me” (Interviewer 8).

    Third, our interview data revealed that interviewers also apply IM to influence outcomes

    related to themselves, such as a strong interviewer reputation (intended IM outcome 6a) and their

    own career advancement (intended IM outcome 6b). So far, interview research has primarily focused

    on outcomes related to the interview’s selection and recruitment purpose (Dipboye et al., 2012), so

    these findings add a new aspect to interview research by stressing interviewers’ aims. Intended

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 32

    interviewer IM outcomes such as interviewer reputation and career advancement indicate that

    interviewers have certain self-centered motives and career goals in mind when they interact with

    applicants.

    When are interviewers’ intended IM outcomes likely to be reached? First, we found that

    not all interviewers intend to achieve all of the outcomes presented in Table 3 and Figure 1. Some

    intended outcomes were reported more than others in our in-depth interviews and thus seem to be

    fundamental to interviewers, such as a strong organizational image and reputation (intended IM

    outcome 3c), job choice as well as recommendation and reapplication intention and behavior

    (intended IM outcomes 4a, 4b, and 4d). In contrast, some intended outcomes were reported only in a

    few in-depth interviews and thus seem more idiosyncratic, such as influencing applicants’ self-

    esteem (intended IM outcome 2b), preventing legal action (intended IM outcome 4e), promoting

    strong interviewer reputation (intended IM outcome 6a), and improving interviewer career

    advancement (intended IM outcome 6b).

    Second, as can be seen in Figure 1, our results indicate that there is a pattern to which IM

    behaviors and outcomes are most closely linked, that is, certain IM behaviors are more closely linked

    to certain outcome components than to others. Interviewers’ IM intentions and behaviors seem to

    differ for short-term versus long-term perspectives regarding their intended recruiting-related

    outcomes. For example, we found that IM behaviors used to signal attractiveness are primarily linked

    to the intended outcome of fast recruiting (filling the vacancy as quickly as possible), whereas IM

    behaviors used to signal authenticity are primarily linked to sustainable recruiting (trying to achieve

    high job tenure).

    Third, the intended recruiting-related outcome of preventing legal action was found to be

    primarily related to IM behaviors that are applied to signal distance in terms of professionalism. In

    addition, selection-related outcomes such as retrieving a high amount of valid personal information

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 33

    from applicants were found to be primarily associated with IM behaviors applied to signal closeness.

    For example, as one interviewer put it “an emotional tie makes the applicant trust in me, so that he

    communicates with me in a transparent way” (Interviewer 6).

    Fourth, the interviewer-related outcome of strong interviewer reputation was found to be

    mainly associated with IM behaviors applied to signal distance in terms of superiority. For instance,

    an interviewer told us, “Well, I’m well known because of my reputation … I’m one of the most

    ruthless ones in our HR department” (Interviewer 2).

    Finally, we found indications that the different intended IM outcomes intertwine in a complex

    pattern. For example, an organization’s strong image and reputation partly depend on how applicants

    experience the interview process and spread the word, such as “When he goes home with positive

    emotions then he’ll tell others about it, he’ll tell his friends and other people he knows, and hopefully

    these others will apply, too” (Interviewer 7). Furthermore, the intended outcome of interviewers’

    career advancement is not depicted in Figure 1 because we found that this component intertwined

    with (and implicit in) other intended interviewer IM outcomes. For instance, if an interviewer

    achieves good acceptance rates and gets positive feedback from applicants, colleagues, and

    supervisors, this should have a favorable impact on the interviewer’s career.

    Discussion

    Previous research on IM in interviews has been fruitful, but this literature has lacked a

    conceptual model to aid in understanding how and why interviewers try to make impressions on

    applicants. Instead, previous work has been based on the assumption that interviewers use the same

    IM behaviors as applicants without acknowledging what intentions and opportunities interviewers

    actually have when they interact with applicants. Thus, as a response to repeated calls for research on

    interviewer IM (e.g., Dipboye & Johnson, 2013; Gilmore et al., 1999; Macan, 2009), our study offers

    a new perspective on the selection interview by systematically examining interviewer IM. Following

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 34

    a grounded theory approach, we identified how interviewers apply IM in terms of what they intend to

    signal to applicants (i.e., interviewer IM intentions) and which signals interviewers deliberately use

    to create their intended impressions (i.e., interviewer IM behaviors). Furthermore, we examined why

    interviewers apply IM in terms of the outcomes they want to achieve by deliberately sending signals

    to applicants (i.e., intended interviewer IM outcomes).

    We developed a conceptual model of interviewer IM that comprises interviewer IM

    intentions, behaviors, and intended outcomes, which also shows patterns of relationships among these

    elements. In addition to the model, we generated an extensive taxonomy of different interviewer IM

    intentions, behaviors, and outcomes. Specifically, we found that interviewers’ primary intentions are

    to signal attractiveness and authenticity, while their secondary intentions are to signal closeness and

    distance (i.e., distance in terms of professionalism and in terms of superiority). Another finding was

    that interviewer IM may have different aims – aims in terms of creating a certain impression of the

    interviewer as a person, an impression of the job, of the team, and of the organization as a whole. In

    order to create these impressions on applicants, interviewers may deliberately apply a broad spectrum

    of signals such as verbal, nonverbal, paraverbal, artifactual, and administrative IM behaviors.

    Additionally, we found that interviewers use IM behaviors in order to improve a wide range of

    different outcomes related to recruitment, selection, and interviewers themselves.

    Implications for Theory

    This study makes at least three important contributions to the literature. First, this study

    elaborates signaling theory (Bangerter et al., 2012; Spence, 1973) in the context of interviewer

    behavior by presenting a conceptual model on the key elements of deliberate signaling processes on

    the part of the interviewer. Notably, our model not only focuses on IM behaviors, but also includes

    interviewer IM intentions, and intended outcomes, which are particularly important to understanding

    the phenomenon of interviewer IM (Dipboye et al., 2012). In addition, as a response to calls to study

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 35

    signals and incentives of signalers within their social context (Connelly et al., 2011), we present an

    extensive taxonomy of the impressions that interviewers aim to create, the signals they deliberately

    use to create these intended impressions, and the outcomes they want to achieve. As such, we found

    that interviewers’ intentions and signals are very broad and complex, and we uncovered numerous

    aspects that clearly go beyond those assumed by previous IM research (see Tables 1 to 3). Regarding

    intended IM outcomes, interviewers deliberately use signaling behaviors not only to enhance

    organizations’ recruitment success and the quality of selection decisions, but also to enhance

    outcomes that are directly related to themselves such as their reputation as an interviewer.

    Second, this study sheds light on how interviewers’ and applicants’ IM are similar and

    distinct. Consistent with interdependence theory (Rusbult & Van Lange, 2003), the present findings

    show that while applicant and interviewer IM share similarities, there is a broad range of differences.

    Similarities can be found, for example, in categories of verbal IM (e.g., fit-focused IM), nonverbal

    IM (e.g., smiling), and artifactual IM behaviors (e.g., modifying one’s appearance). However, in

    contrast to assumptions in previous studies (e.g., Stevens et al., 1990), many interviewer IM

    intentions and behaviors seem to be distinct from those of applicants. Unlike applicants, interviewers

    may have multiple aims of IM (e.g., influencing applicant impressions of the job, the organization,

    and themselves) and may have diverse IM intentions that go well beyond mere friendliness (e.g.,

    signaling distance).

    Another difference between interviewer and applicant IM is that, because interviewers are in a

    more powerful position than applicants, they may apply IM behaviors such as applicant-depreciation

    and challenging applicants to signal their superiority. Also, as another consequence of interviewers’

    more powerful position, they have a greater freedom of action than applicants and are therefore able

    to control and modify diverse artifactual (e.g., providing giveaways) and administrative aspects of the

    interview (e.g., inviting the applicant personally) to favorably influence applicant impressions.

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 36

    Therefore, because of these differences, interviewer IM should be considered a phenomenon that may

    be related to, but is nevertheless quite distinct from applicant IM.

    Finally, our results suggest a shift in the way that we think about interviewers in the

    employment interview. For instance, our study draws attention to the social nature of the interview

    and contributes to a more person-centric picture of the interviewer (following suggestions by Weiss

    & Rupp, 2011). We found that interviewers are well aware that they may influence applicant

    impressions and explicitly state their aims to do so. Interviewers know very well which specific

    impressions they want applicants to form and intentionally use a broad range of different signals to

    create these intended impressions. Our findings support efforts by other researchers to enhance the

    theoretical understanding and the quality of the interview as an assessment tool by acknowledging

    social exchange processes in the interview, such as interviewer IM (cf. Dipboye et al., 2012).

    Potential Limitations

    Although this study provides valuable insights into how and why interviewers intentionally

    try to create impressions on applicants, it has its limitations. This study included a range of different

    interview formats, which allowed us to capture a broad range of IM behaviors. However, by the same

    token, we did not focus on one individual type of format (e.g., panel interviews), which would have

    allowed for more extensive insights about IM behavior within a particular interview format.

    However, we believe that the IM behaviors presented in this study that refer to specific interview

    formats (e.g., IM behaviors applied in panel interviews) constitute important initial findings.

    Another limitation is that even though the application of a qualitative approach can lead to

    new research questions and new perspectives (Cassell & Symon, 2011), the generalizability of the

    findings might be limited because of small sample sizes (Lee, Mitchell, & Sablynski, 1999).

    Moreover, the present study was conducted in Germany and Switzerland. Interviewer IM intentions

    and behaviors may vary between different national cultures, as it has been found for applicant IM

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 37

    (e.g., König, Hafsteinsson, Jansen, & Stadelmann, 2011). Hence, more research on interviewer IM

    with additional samples is clearly needed. However, the present study sampled a broad range of

    interviewers and applicants, included different interview formats (face-to-face, telephone, video, one-

    on-one, and panel interviews), and used multiple qualitative methods (in-depth interviews,

    observations, memos, and analyses of informational material) to generate a comprehensive taxonomy

    of interviewer IM, thus providing insights into associations between interviewer IM intentions,

    behaviors, and intended outcomes. In addition, data were collected until theoretical saturation was

    reached (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Taken together, the diversity of samples and methods and the

    achievement of theoretical saturation suggest that these results should generalize to other interview

    contexts.

    Implications and Propositions for Future Research

    The goal of our conceptual model is to provide a framework for future research on interviewer

    IM, ultimately leading to practical recommendations to organizations and interviewers. Thus, we

    believe that the initial qualitative findings presented in this paper should be bolstered by insightful

    future research before such practical recommendations can be convincingly made. As can be seen in

    Table 4, the conceptual model of interviewer IM presented in this study provides a promising

    blueprint for future research in at least three different ways: 1) testing the conceptual model as it is

    presented in this paper in terms of the elements and relationships within the model, 2) expanding the

    conceptual model on the basis of this study, that is, integrating further factors and relationships that

    were indicated by our data and mentioned in our results, and 3) expanding the conceptual model

    beyond the scope of our study, that is, connecting the model to ongoing discussions in the literature.

    In Table 4 and in the following sections, we point out promising paths for future research and provide

    specific ideas for research propositions for each of these three research paths. It is important to note

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 38

    that the research topics and propositions that we present are intended to be illustrative and stimulating

    rather than all-inclusive.

    On a first level, future research should focus on the elements and associations in our

    conceptual model. Each relationship within the model constitutes an actionable proposition for future

    research that could be tested using quantitative methods. Specifically, the model can be tested in

    terms of the way the boxes of IM intentions are arranged (i.e., structure of IM intentions) and the way

    the cells of IM behaviors are embedded within or are bridging boxes of IM intentions (i.e.,

    relationships between IM intentions and IM behaviors). Accordingly, researchers could also examine

    whether the model as developed from the qualitative methods in this study translates into a factor

    structure as determined through quantitative research. In addition, each arrow in Figure 1 that points

    from IM behaviors and their underlying intentions to intended IM outcomes might be tested (i.e.,

    relationships between IM behaviors and intended IM outcomes).

    As a second research path, future research should add additional aspects to the conceptual

    model of interviewer IM that have been suggested by our data. One idea to expand the model would

    be to integrate the higher-level category structure of IM behaviors that we found (see Table 2). It

    would be worthwhile to examine how the five types of IM behaviors (i.e., verbal, paraverbal,

    nonverbal, artifactual, and administrative) are linked to primary and secondary IM intentions. For

    example, our data suggest that paraverbal and nonverbal IM behaviors may play a particularly

    important role for expressing secondary IM intentions because they represent indirect ways of

    communicating, which might be especially important for interviewers’ personal interactions with

    applicants. Another example would be research on whether intentions as expressed by the interviewer

    translate into behaviors as observed by the interviewee. Additional promising themes and

    propositions for future research that are suggested by our data refer to the question of how

    interviewer IM may depend on the industry sector (e.g., industry sector’s image and the types of

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 39

    services provided), the timing in the interview (e.g., beginning or end of the interview), and the

    content of the conversation (e.g., asking delicate vs. challenging questions).

    As a third path, future research should connect the conceptual model of interviewer IM to

    ongoing discussions in the literature, such as how IM relates to truthfulness in terms of honest vs.

    deceptive IM and how the validity of interviews might be affected by IM. Regarding the discussion

    on IM and truthfulness, we believe that it would be helpful to weave an additional conceptual layer

    into our conceptual model in terms of honest versus deceptive interviewer IM. In line with recent

    suggestions by Tsai and Huang (2014), we believe that interviewer IM can be honest or deceptive

    depending on whether the signal being sent to an applicant relates to an existing attribute of the

    interviewer, the job, or the organization instead of being misleading in terms of creating false

    impressions. We see high potential in honest interviewer IM for increasing long-term outcomes that

    are highly relevant for organizations and their employees. For example, our data indicate that honest

    IM can stress positive attributes of the job and the organization while at the same time creating a

    realistic image on the applicants’ minds. In contrast, we believe that there is a risk in deceptive

    interviewer IM by leading to unrealistic expectations and psychological contract breach, which

    should result in negative long-term consequences for organizations and their employees such as low

    levels of job satisfaction, performance, and tenure.

    Regarding the ongoing discussion on potential effects of IM on interview validity, one

    possibility is that interviewers’ attempts to influence applicants’ impressions might prevent

    interviewers from accurately assessing applicant performance (Dipboye et al., 2012). As such, Marr

    and Cable (2014) found that interviewers’ selling orientation reduced the accuracy and predictive

    validity of their judgments. However, our data indicate an additional possibility, namely that

    interviewer IM may also facilitate the quality of selection decisions. Specifically, we found initial

    evidence that interviewer IM behavior such as demonstrating empathy may facilitate effective

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 40

    selection by enhancing feelings of trust and reciprocity, which may encourage applicants to open up

    and provide not only more but also more honest personal information. This would provide

    interviewers with a better basis for performance assessment and would thus enhance valid selection

    decisions. We believe that these potential positive impacts on interview validity are well worth

    further examination.

    In sum, we hope that future research will provide further confirmation and refinement of the

    qualitative insights gained in this study. Specifically, we suggest that future research should drill

    down further in the direction of both the numerous potential risks and opportunities that are involved

    in interviewer IM, hopefully inspired by the research propositions presented. If future research

    follows these paths, our theoretical understanding of interviewer IM can be substantially enhanced.

    Importantly, this future research will facilitate practical recommendations that will move interviewers

    closer to successfully selecting and recruiting applicants.

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 41

    References

    Anderson, N. R. (1992). Eight decades of employment interview research: A retrospective meta-

    review and prospective commentary. European Work and Organizational Psychologist, 2, 1-

    32. doi:10.1080/09602009208408532

    Bangerter, A., Roulin, N., & König, C. J. (2012). Personnel selection as a signaling game. Journal of

    Applied Psychology, 97, 719–738. doi:10.1037/a0026078

    Bansal, P., & Corley, K. (2011). The coming of age for qualitative research: Embracing the diversity

    of qualitative methods. Academy of Management Journal, 54, 233-237.

    doi:10.5465/AMJ.2011.60262792

    Barrick, M. R., Shaffer, J. A., & DeGrassi, S. W. (2009). What you see may not be what you get:

    Relationships among self-presentation tactics and ratings of interview and job performance.

    Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 1394-1411. doi:10.1037/A0016532

    Bluhm, D. J., Harman, W., Lee, T. W., & Mitchell, T. R. (2011). Qualitative research in

    management: A decade of progress. Journal of Management Studies, 48, 1866-1891.

    doi:10.1111/j.1467-6486.2010.00972.x

    Bodie, G. D. (2011). The active-empathic listening scale (AELS): Conceptualization and evidence of

    validity within the interpersonal domain. Communication Quarterly, 59, 277-295.

    doi:10.1080/01463373.2011.583495

    Bolino, M. C., Kacmar, K. M., Turnley, W. H., & Gilstrap, J. B. (2008). A multi-level review of

    impression management motives and behaviors. Journal of Management, 34, 1080-1109.

    doi:10.1177/0149206308324325

    Butterfield, K. D., Trevino, L. K., & Ball, G. A. (1996). Punishment from the manager’s perspective:

    A grounded investigation and inductive model. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 1479-

    1512. doi:10.2307/257066

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 42

    Carless, S. A., & Hetherington, K. (2011). Understanding the applicant recruitment experience: Does

    timeliness matter? International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 19, 105-108.

    doi:10.1111/j.1468-2389.2010.00538.x

    Carless, S. A., & Imber, A. (2007). The influence of perceived interviewer and job and organizational

    characteristics on applicant attraction and job choice intentions: The role of applicant anxiety.

    International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 15, 359-371. doi:10.1111/j.1468-

    2389.2007.00395.x

    Cassell, C., & Symon, G. (2011). Assessing ‘good’ qualitative research in the work psychology field:

    A narrative analysis. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 84, 633-650.

    doi:10.1111/j.2044-8325.2011.02009.x

    Celani, A., & Singh, P. (2011). Signaling theory and applicant attraction outcomes. Personnel

    Review, 40, 222-238. doi:10.1108/00483481111106093

    Chapman, D. S., Uggerslev, K. L., Carroll, S. A., Piasentin, K. A., & Jones, D. A. (2005). Applicant

    attraction to organizations and job choice: A meta-analytic review of the correlates of

    recruiting outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 928-944. doi:10.1037/0021-

    9010.90.5.928

    Connelly, B. L., Certo, S. T., Ireland, R. D., & Reutzel, C. R. (2011). Signaling theory: A review and

    assessment. Journal of Management, 37, 39-67. doi:10.1177/0149206310388419

    Connerley, M. L., & Rynes, S. L. (1997). The influence of recruiter characteristics and organizational

    recruitment support on perceived recruiter effectiveness: Views from applicants and

    recruiters. Human Relations, 50, 1563-1586. doi:10.1023/A:1016923732255

    Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. L. (2008). Basics of qualitative research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 43

    DeGroot, T., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1999). Why visual and vocal interview cues can affect interviewers’

    judgments and predict job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 986-993.

    doi:10.1037/0021-9010.84.6.986

    Delery, J. E., & Kacmar, K. M. (1998). The influence of applicant and interviewer characteristics on

    the use of impression management. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1649-1669.

    doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01339.x

    Derous, E. (2007). Investigating personnel selection from a counseling perspective: Do applicants’

    and recruiters’ perceptions correspond? Journal of Employment Counseling, 44, 60-72.

    doi:10.1002/j.2161-1920.2007.tb00025.x

    Dipboye, R. L. (2005). The selection/recruitment interview: Core processes and contexts. In A.

    Evers, N. Anderson & O. Voskuijl (Eds.), The Blackwell handbook of personnel selection (pp.

    121-142). Malden, CA:

    Blackwell.

    Dipboye, R. L., & Johnson, S. K. (2013). Understanding and improving employee selection

    interviews. In K. F. Geisinger (Ed.), APA handbook of testing and assessment in psychology

    (pp. 479-499). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Dipboye, R. L., Macan, T. H., & Shahani-Denning, C. (2012). The selection interview from the

    interviewer and applicant perspectives: Can’t have one without the other. In N. Schmitt (Ed.),

    The Oxford handbook of personnel assessment and selection (pp. 323-352). New York, NY:

    Oxford University Press.

    Earnest, D. R., Allen, D. G., & Landis, R. S. (2011). Mechanisms linking realistic job previews with

    turnover: A meta-analytic path analysis. Personnel Psychology, 64, 865-897.

    doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2011.01230.x

    Eisenhardt, K. M., & Graebner, M. E. (2007). Theory building from cases: Opportunities and

    challenges. Academy of Management Journal, 50, 25-32. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2007.24160888

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 44

    Ellis, A. P. J., West, B. J., Ryan, A. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2002). The use of impression management

    tactics in structured interviews: A function of question type? Journal of Applied Psychology,

    87, 1200-1208. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.87.6.1200

    Fleiss, J. L., & Cohen, J. (1973). The equivalence of weighted kappa and the intraclass correlation

    coefficient as measures of reliability. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 33, 613-

    619. doi:10.1177/001316447303300309

    Friese, S. (2011). User’s manual for ATLAS.ti 6. Berlin, Germany: Scientific Software Development.

    Gardner, W. L., & Martinko, M. J. (1988). Impression management in organizations. Journal of

    Management, 14, 321-338. doi:10.1177/014920638801400210

    Gilmore, D. C., Stevens, C. K., Harrel-Cook, G., & Ferris, G. R. (1999). Impression management

    tactics. In R. W. Eder & M. M. Harris (Eds.), The employment interview handbook (pp. 321-

    336). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative

    research. New York, NY: Aldine.

    Grzywacz, J. G., Arcury, T. A., Marín, A., Carrillo, L., Burke, B., Coates, M. L., et al. (2007). Work-

    family conflict: Experiences and health implications among immigrant Latinos. Journal of

    Applied Psychology, 92, 1119-1130. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.4.1119

    Hausknecht, J. P., Day, D. V., & Thomas, S. C. (2004). Applicant reactions to selection procedures:

    An updated model and meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 57, 639-683.

    doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2004.00003.x

    Hornby, A. S., & Wehmeier, S. (2005). Oxford advanced learner’s dictionary of current English (7th

    ed.). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Jones, E. E., & Pittman, T. S. (1982). Toward a general theory of strategic self-presentation. In J.

    Suls (Ed.), Psychological perspectives on the self (pp. 231-262). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 45

    Kazdin, A. E. (1977). Artifact, bias, and complexity of assessment: The ABCs of reliability. Journal

    of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 141-150. doi:10.1901/jaba.1977.10-141

    König, C. J., Hafsteinsson, L. G., Jansen, A., & Stadelmann, E. H. (2011). Applicants’ self-

    presentational behavior across cultures: Less self-presentation in Switzerland and Iceland than

    in the United States. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 19, 331-339.

    doi:10.1111/j.1468-2389.2011.00562.x

    Koslowsky, M., & Pindek, S. (2011). Impression management: Influencing perceptions of self. In D.

    Chadee (Ed.), Theories in social psychology (pp. 280-296). Chichester, England: Wiley-

    Blackwell.

    Kreiner, G. E., Hollensbe, E. C., & Sheep, M. L. (2009). Balancing borders and bridges: Negotiating

    the work-home interface via boundary work tactics. Academy of Management Journal, 52,

    704-730. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2009.43669916

    Lee, T. W., Mitchell, T. R., & Sablynski, C. J. (1999). Qualitative research in organizational and

    vocational psychology, 1979-1999. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 55, 161-187.

    doi:10.1006/jvbe.1999.1707

    Levashina, J., & Campion, M. A. (2007). Measuring faking in the employment interview:

    Development and validation of an Interview Faking Behavior scale. Journal of Applied

    Psychology, 92, 1638-1656. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.6.1638

    Levashina, J., Hartwell, C. J., Morgeson, F. P., & Campion, M. A. (2014). The structured

    employment interview: Narrative and quantitative review of the research literature. Personnel

    Psychology, 67, 241-293. doi:10.1111/peps.12052

    Levine, S. P., & Feldman, R. S. (2002). Women and men’s nonverbal behavior and self-monitoring in

    a job interview setting. Applied H.R.M. Research, 7, 1-14.

    Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (Eds.). (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 46

    Locke, K. (2001). Grounded theory in management research. London, England: Sage.

    Macan, T. H. (2009). The employment interview: A review of current studies and directions for

    future research. Human Resource Management Review, 19, 203-218.

    doi:10.1016/j.hrmr.2009.03.006

    Marr, J. C., & Cable, D. M. (2014). Do interviewers sell themselves short? The effects of selling

    orientation on interviewers’ judgements. Academy of Management Journal, 57, 624-651.

    doi:10.5465/amj.2011.0504

    Martin, P. Y., & Turner, B. A. (1986). Grounded theory and organizational research. Journal of

    Applied Behavioral Science, 22, 141-157. doi:10.1177/002188638602200207

    McFarland, L. A., Yun, G., Harold, C. M., Viera, L., Jr., & Moore, L. G. (2005). An examination of

    impression management use and effectiveness across assessment center exercises: The role of

    competency demands. Personnel Psychology, 58, 949-980. doi:10.1111/j.1744-

    6570.2005.00374.x

    Peeters, H., & Lievens, F. (2006). Verbal and nonverbal impression management tactics in behavior

    description and situational interviews. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 14,

    206-222. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2389.2006.00348.x

    Rosenfeld, P. (1997). Impression management, fairness, and the employment interview. Journal of

    Business Ethics, 16, 801-808. doi:10.1023/A:1017972627516

    Rusbult, C. E., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2003). Interdependence, interaction, and relationships.

    Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 351-375. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145059

    Schilling, J. (2006). On the pragmatics of qualitative assessment: Designing the process for content

    analysis. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 22, 28-37. doi:10.1027/1015-

    5759.22.1.28

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 47

    Schlenker, B. R. (1980). Impression Management: The self-concept, social identity, and

    interpersonal relations. Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole.

    Schneider, D. J. (1981). Tactical self-presentations: Toward a broader conception. In J. T. Tedeschi

    (Ed.), Impression management theory and social psychological research (pp. 23-40). New

    York, NY: Academic Press.

    Spence, M. (1973). Job market signaling. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 87, 355-374.

    doi:10.2307/1882010

    Stevens, C. K., Mitchell, T. R., & Tripp, T. M. (1990). Order of presentation and verbal recruitment

    strategy effectiveness. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 20, 1076-1092.

    doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1990.tb00391.x

    Suddaby, R. (2006). From the editors: What grounded theory is not. Academy of Management

    Journal, 49, 633-642. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2006.22083020

    Tsai, W.-C., & Huang, T.-C. (2014). Impression Management during the Recruitment Process. In K.

    Y. T. Yu & D. M. Cable (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of recruitment (pp. 314-334). New

    York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Tullar, W. L. (1989). Relational control in the employment interview. Journal of Applied Psychology,

    74, 971-977. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.74.6.971

    Van Iddekinge, C. H., McFarland, L. A., & Raymark, P. H. (2007). Antecedents of impression

    management use and effectiveness in a structured interview. Journal of Management, 33,

    752-773. doi:10.1177/0149206307305563

    Wanous, J. P. (1976). Organizational entry: From naive expectations to realistic beliefs. Journal of

    Applied Psychology, 61, 22-29. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.61.1.22

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 48

    Weiss, H. M., & Rupp, D. E. (2011). Experiencing work: An essay on a person-centric work

    psychology. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 4, 83-97. doi:10.1111/j.1754-

    9434.2010.01302.x

    Willig, C. (2009) Introducing qualitative research in psychology: Adventures in theory and mehtod

    (2nd ed.). Maidenhead, England: Open University Press.

    Yanow, D., & Schwartz-Shea, P. (2006). Interpretation and method: Empirical research methods

    and the interpretive turn. New York, NY: M.E. Sharpe.

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 49

    Table 1

    How Interviewers Apply IM: Structure of Interviewer IM Intentions

    Higher-level categories Lower-level categories

    Primary IM intentions: What do interviewers intend to signal to
    applicants with regard to representing the organization?

    1. Attractiveness

    2. Authenticity

    Secondary IM intentions: What do interviewers intend to signal
    to applicants with regard to their personal interaction with the
    applicant?

    3. Closeness 3a. Building rapport
    3b. Individuality and appreciation
    3c. Trustworthiness

    4. Distance in terms of professionalism 4a. Fairness
    4b. Selection complexity and effort
    4c. Straightforwardness

    5. Distance in terms of superiority 5a. Status and power of decision
    5b. Performance expectations
    5c. Suspense

    Note. Categories of interviewer IM intentions that are printed in italics are new in comparison to Barrick et al. (2009) and
    Jones and Pittman (1982).

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 50

    Table 2

    How Interviewers Apply IM: Structure of Interviewer IM Behaviors

    Higher-level categories Lower-level categories

    Verbal IM behaviors: What do interviewers deliberately
    say to influence applicant impressions?

    1. Self-focused 1a. Self-enhancement
    1b. Demonstrating job knowledge
    1c. Demonstrating humor
    1d. Telling personal stories
    1e. Expressing enthusiasm

    2. Applicant-focused 2a. Referring to applicant by name
    2b. Demonstrating knowledge of the applicant
    2c. Applicant-enhancement
    2d. Goal setting for the applicant
    2e. Demonstrating empathy
    2f. Thanking
    2g. Offering support
    2h. Giving voice
    2i. Challenging
    2j. Applicant-depreciation

    3. Fit-focused 3a. Fit enhancing
    3b. Demonstrating similarity

    4. Job-, team-, or organization-focused 4a. Enhancement of job, team, or organization
    4b. Goal setting for the job, team, or

    organization
    4c. Confessing
    4d. Positive framing

    5. Interview process-focused 5a. Enhancement of the interview process
    5b. Apologizing

    6. Through style of communication 6a. Paraphrasing and summarizing
    6b. Verbal encouragement
    6c. Modifying applicant’s speech portion
    6d. Modifying one’s detailedness of language
    6e. Modifying one’s formality of language
    6f. Adapting one’s vocabulary and dialect

    Paraverbal IM behaviors: How do interviewers
    deliberately use their voice to influence applicant
    impressions?

    7. Speaking in an empathetic way 7a. Speaking with low pace
    7b. Speaking with low volume
    7c. Speaking with high pitch

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 51

    8. Speaking in an authoritative way 8a. Speaking with high pace
    8b. Speaking with high volume
    8c. Speaking with low pitch

    9. Speaking in an unobtrusive 9a. Speaking with moderate pace
    9b. Speaking with moderate volume
    9c. Speaking with moderate pitch

    Nonverbal IM behaviors: How do interviewers deliberately
    use body language to influence applicant impressions?

    10. Towards the applicant 10a. Laughing
    10b. Smiling
    10c. Nodding affirmatively
    10d. Making eye contact

    10e. Making hand gestures
    10f. Leaning forward
    10g. Mirroring
    10h. Note taking
    10i. Shaking hands
    10j. Backslapping
    10k. Doing something else

    11. Towards other interviewers 11a. Smiling
    11b. Nodding affirmatively
    11c. Mirroring

    Artifactual IM behaviors: How do interviewers
    deliberately use appearance, visual information, and
    promotional items to influence applicant impressions?

    12. Through interviewer appearance 12a. Modifying one’s clothing
    12b. Modifying one’s accessories

    13. Through premises appearance 13a. Choosing the interview building
    13b. Choosing the interview room
    13c. Decorating the interview room
    13d. Checking the light intensity
    13e. Choosing the interview table
    13f. Choosing the seating furniture
    13g. Placement of seating furniture

    14. Through visual information 14a. Showing printed information material
    14b. Displaying application documents
    14c. Displaying notes taken prior to the

    interview
    14d. Displaying test results

    15. Through promotional items 15a. Handing out printed information material
    15b. Handing out promotional gifts
    15c. Handing out one’s business card

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 52

    Administrative IM behaviors: How do interviewers
    deliberately use timing of communication and provide
    services to influence applicant impressions?

    16. Through timing of communication 16a. Ensuring timeliness of pre-interview
    communication

    16b. Modifying timeliness of interview start
    16c. Modifying interview length
    16d. Ensuring timeliness of feedback
    16e. Offering time to think the offer over

    17. By providing services to applicants before the
    interview

    17a. Confirming receipt of application
    17b. Giving directions
    17c. Inviting the applicant personally
    17d. Inviting the applicant by telephone
    17e. Accommodating with the interview date
    17f. Accommodating with the interview location
    17g. Preventing interruptions
    17h. Modifying the room temperature
    17i. Airing the interview room

    18. By providing services to applicants during the
    interview

    18a. Approaching the applicant
    18b. Taking the applicant’s jacket
    18c. Offering drinks
    18d. Offering a break
    18e. Incorporating future colleagues
    18f. Offering a site visit
    18g. Offering refund of travel expenses
    18h. Escorting

    19. By providing services to applicants after the interview 19a. Giving feedback personally
    19b. Giving feedback orally
    19c. Giving detailed feedback

    Note. Categories of interviewer IM behaviors that are printed in italics are new in comparison to Barrick et al. (2009),
    Bolino et al. (2008), DeGroot and Motowidlo (1999), Ellis et al. (2002), Levashina and Campion (2007), McFarland et al.
    (2005), Peeters and Lievens (2006), and Schneider (1981).

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 53

    Table 3

    Why Interviewers Apply IM: Structure of Intended IM Outcomes

    Higher-level categories Lower-level categories

    Recruitment-related IM Outcomes: What outcomes in
    terms of the interview’s recruitment function do
    interviewers want to achieve by deliberately sending
    signals to applicants?

    1. Organizations’ recruiting success 1a. Fast recruiting
    1b. Sustainable recruiting

    2. Applicants’ positive emotions 2a. Positive affective state
    2b. Self-esteem

    3. Applicants’ positive attitudes towards the
    organization

    3a. Job-organization attraction
    3b. Identification with the organization

    3c. Strong organizational image and reputation

    4. Applicants’ positive intentions and behaviors
    towards the organization

    4a. Job choice intention and behavior
    4b. Recommendation intention and behavior

    4c. Consumer intention and behavior
    4d. Reapplication intention and behavior
    4e. Prevention of legal action

    Selection-related IM Outcomes: What outcomes in
    terms of the interview’s selection function do
    interviewers want to achieve by deliberately sending
    signals to applicants?

    5. Information disclosed by applicants 5a. Amount of personal information disclosed
    5b. Informative value of personal information

    Interviewer-related IM Outcomes: What outcomes in
    terms of self-centered motives do interviewers want to
    achieve by deliberately sending signals to applicants?

    6. Interviewers’ career 6a. Strong interviewer reputation
    6b. Interviewer career advancement

    Note. Categories of intended interviewer IM outcomes that are printed in italics are new in comparison to Chapman et al.
    (2005) and Hausknecht et al. (2004).

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 54

    Table 4

    Examples of Propositions for Future Research

    Level 1: Testing relationships within the conceptual model of interviewer IM (see Figure 1)

    Examples of research issues Examples of specific propositions
    Structure of IM intentions P1-1 Five kinds of interviewer IM intentions can be distinguished,

    which differ regarding their interrelations: intentions that are
    located closer together in the figure co-occur more often than
    intentions that are located further apart.

    P1-2 There are two major themes of interviewer IM intentions: (a)
    primary IM intentions that refer to interviewers’ goal of
    representing the organization, the job, and themselves, and (b)
    secondary IM intentions that refer to interviewers’ personal
    interaction with the applicant.

    Relationships between IM intentions and IM
    behaviors

    P1-3 There are three kinds of IM behaviors that differ in how they
    are related to IM intentions: (a) behaviors that are most
    strongly related to one single IM intention, (b) behaviors that
    are most strongly related to two IM intentions, and (c)
    behaviors that are multipurpose, that is, they are related to
    different IM intentions depending on how they are applied.

    Relationships between IM behaviors and
    intended IM outcomes

    P1-4 IM behaviors used to signal attractiveness are related to
    intended short-term outcomes such as fast recruiting, whereas
    IM behaviors used to signal authenticity are related to intended
    long-term outcomes such as sustainable recruitment.

    Level 2: Expanding the conceptual model of interviewer IM on the basis of this study

    Relationships between the five types of IM
    behavior and IM intentions

    P2-1 Paraverbal and nonverbal IM behaviors are more strongly
    related to secondary IM intentions than to primary IM
    intentions, whereas verbal, artifactual, and administrative IM
    behaviors are related to both primary and secondary IM
    intentions.

    Influence of industry sector P2-2 The specific set of IM intentions, behaviors, and intended
    outcomes depends on the industry sector such as the industry
    sector’s image and the types of services provided.

    Influence of timing P2-3 The IM intention of signaling closeness is more predominant in
    the beginning and at the end of interviews.

    Influence of interview content P2-4 The IM intention of signaling closeness is more predominant
    when interviewers ask delicate questions, whereas signaling
    distance in terms of superiority is more predominant when
    interviewers ask challenging questions.

    Level 3: Expanding the conceptual model of interviewer IM beyond the scope of this study

    Honest vs. deceptive IM P3-1 Deceptive interviewer IM decreases long-term outcomes such
    as employees’ job satisfaction, job performance, and job tenure
    through unrealistic job expectations and psychological contract
    breach.

    Interview validity P3-2 Interviewer IM of signaling closeness increases (a) the amount
    and (b) the informative value of personal information provided
    by applicants, which, in turn, positively influences interview
    validity.

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 55

    In
    te
    nd

    ed
    IM

     O
    ut
    co
    m
    es

    IM behaviors to create
    an attractive image, 
    e.g.
    • Expressing 
    enthusiasm

    • Decorating the 
    interview room

    Attractiveness

    IM behaviors to create
    an attractive and
    authentic image, e.g. 
    • Positive framing
    • Demonstrating job 
    knowledge

    IM behaviors to create
    a close relation image, 
    e.g.
    • Demonstrating 
    similarity

    • Laughing

    Authenticity

    Closeness Distance in terms of professionalism Distance in terms of superiority

    IM behaviors to create
    a professional image, 
    e.g.
    • Displaying application 
    documents 

    • Note taking

    IM behaviors to create
    a superior image, e.g.
    • Applicant‐
    depreciation

    • Speaking in an 
    authoritative way

    IM behaviors to create
    an authentic and
    superior image, e.g.
    • Giving voice
    • Incorporating future 
    colleagues

    IM behaviors to create
    a professional and
    authentic image, e.g.
    • Goal setting for the 
    applicant

    • Offering a site visit

    IM behaviors to create
    an attractive and
    professional image, e.g.
    • Demonstrating 
    knowledge of the
    applicant

    • Offering support

    IM behaviors to create
    an attractive and close
    relation image, e.g.
    • Demonstrating 
    humor

    • Shaking hands

    IM behaviors to create
    a close relation and
    professional image, e.g.
    • Inviting the applicant 
    by telephone

    • Making eye contact

    IM behavior to create a 
    professional and
    superior image
    • Challenging

    IM behaviors that can
    be used in a 
    multipurposeway, e.g. 
    • Modifying applicants’ 
    speech portion

    • Placement of seating 
    furniture

    IM behaviors to create
    an authentic image, e.g.
    • Confessing
    • Mirroring

    Pr
    im

    ar

    IM

     In
    te
    nt
    io
    ns

    Se
    co
    nd

    ar
    y
    IM

     In
    te
    nt
    io
    ns

    Amount of personal information 
    disclosed, informative value of personal 
    information disclosed

    Applicants’ positive affective state, 
    applicants’ self‐esteem

    Prevention of legal action Strong interviewer reputation

    Fast recruiting
    Job‐organization attraction
    Job choice intention and behavior, recommendation intention and 
    behavior, consumer intention and behavior, reapplication intention 
    and behavior

    Sustainable recruiting
    Identification with the organization, strong 
    organizational image and reputation 

    Figure 1. Conceptual model of how interviewers apply impression management (IM) in terms of IM intentions (light and dark gray, square boxes) and IM behaviors
    (white, round-edged cells), and why interviewers apply IM in terms of intended IM outcomes (at the end of arrows outside of boxes). Behaviors (cells) are within or
    touching intentions (boxes) to indicate association. The IM behaviors presented in this figure are not comprehensive, but constitute representative examples of the
    links that were found between IM intentions, behaviors, and intended outcomes gleaned from Tables 1 to 3 to demonstrate the main patterns of these links.

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 56

    Appendix A

    Interview guide for in-depth interviews with interviewers

    Examples of questions asked throughout the whole research process

    (a) In what ways are the kinds of impressions applicants form during your employment interviews important
    to you?

    (b) Could you please describe the specific types of impressions you want applicants to form during your
    employment interviews?

    (c) Would you please tell me how you behave during your employment interviews in order to create these
    impressions on applicants?

    (d) What outcomes can applicants’ impressions lead to?

    Examples of questions that were added later in the research process based on prior in-depth interviews
    and observations

     What kind of information about yourself do you present to create your intended impressions on
    applicants?

     How do you welcome applicants to create your intended impressions?

     Could you please tell me how you present negative aspects of the job or the organization to create your
    intended impressions on applicants?

     Would you please tell me how you interact with applicants after the interview to create your intended
    impressions?

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 57

    Appendix B

    Interview guide for in-depth interviews with applicants

    Examples of questions asked throughout the whole research process

    (a) What kinds of impressions did you form during employment interviews you have recently participated in?

    (b) What specific types of impressions do you think the interviewers might have wanted you to form?

    (c) What interviewer behaviors did you observe that might have been intended to influence the impressions
    you formed?

    (d) What outcomes can applicants’ impressions lead to?
    Examples of questions that were added later in the research process based on prior in-depth interviews
    and observations

     What kind of personal information did the interviewers reveal that might have been intended to influence
    the impressions you formed?

     How did the interviewers welcome you?

     Could you tell me how the interviewers presented negative aspects of the job or the organization?

     What interviewer behaviors did you observe after the interview that might have been intended to influence
    the impressions you formed?

    INTERVIEWER IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT 58

    Appendix C

    Observation guide used to observe actual employment interviews

    Instruction: Please write down any actions you see and statements and questions you hear that may be relevant
    in terms of how and why interviewers apply IM. Please make sure to include specific examples (e.g.,
    direct quotes). The following headings are meant to focus your attention on important aspects. However,
    please add further observations wherever applicable.

    Examples of aspects documented throughout the whole research process

     Date
     Location
     Number of interviewers
     Type of job vacancy
     Duration of the interview

    (a) Observations prior to the employment interview

     Verbal, e.g. what interviewers say and ask
     Paraverbal, e.g., how interviewers talk to applicants
     Nonverbal, e.g. body language of interviewers
     Any other aspect that may be worth further exploration in future observations and in-depth interviews

    (b) Observations during the employment interview

     Verbal, e.g. what interviewers say and ask
     Paraverbal, e.g., how interviewers talk to applicants
     Nonverbal, e.g. body language of interviewers
     Any other aspects that may be worth further exploration in future observations and in-depth interviews

    (c) Observations after the employment interview

     Verbal, e.g. what interviewers say and ask
     Paraverbal, e.g., how interviewers talk to applicants
     Nonverbal, e.g. body language of interviewers
     Any other aspects that may be worth further exploration in future observations and in-depth interviews

    Examples of additional aspects that observers were asked to consider and document later in the research
    process (based on prior observations and in-depth interviews)

    (a) Observations prior to the employment interview

     Administrative, e.g. timeliness of the interview start

    (b) Observations during the employment interview

     Administrative, e.g. refreshments offered
     Artifactual, e.g. seating arrangement, objects visible on the interview table

    (c) Observations after the employment interview

     Administrative, e.g. feedback to applicants

    View publication statsView publication stats

    https://www.researchgate.net/publication/279955332

    Calculate your order
    Pages (275 words)
    Standard price: $0.00
    Client Reviews
    4.9
    Sitejabber
    4.6
    Trustpilot
    4.8
    Our Guarantees
    100% Confidentiality
    Information about customers is confidential and never disclosed to third parties.
    Original Writing
    We complete all papers from scratch. You can get a plagiarism report.
    Timely Delivery
    No missed deadlines – 97% of assignments are completed in time.
    Money Back
    If you're confident that a writer didn't follow your order details, ask for a refund.

    Calculate the price of your order

    You will get a personal manager and a discount.
    We'll send you the first draft for approval by at
    Total price:
    $0.00
    Power up Your Academic Success with the
    Team of Professionals. We’ve Got Your Back.
    Power up Your Study Success with Experts We’ve Got Your Back.

    Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code ESSAYHELP