Week 2-Psychology Method Comparison Journal Exercise

 

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

In this journal exercise, you will observe and reflect on the similarities and differences in two published research studies (one qualitative, one quantitative) on a similar topic (See

Patients’ Perceptions of Barriers to Self-managing Bipolar Disorder: A Qualitative Study

and

Social Support and Relationship Satisfaction in Bipolar Disorder

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

). A table is provided for entering your observations. Prior to beginning this journal assignment, review the required resources for this week and read the two research studies. You may find it helpful to print out the studies and view them side by side, or if you have a large computer screen have them both open to facilitate comparing their features. Download the

Method Comparison Journal Exercise Form

and save it to your computer. Fill in your name and the date, then the cells of the table with your thoughts on the characteristics of the articles. Save your entries and upload the completed file to Waypoint.

Carefully review the 

Grading Rubric (Links to an external site.)

for the criteria that will be used to evaluate your assignment.

Name:

Date:

Method Comparison Journal Exercise

Read the two research articles cited below and fill in what you notice about their characteristics, similarities or differences in the table below.

Qualitative Study:

Blixen, C., Perzynski, A. T., Bukah, A., Howland, M., & Sajatovic, M. (2016). Patients’ perceptions of barriers to self-managing bipolar disorder: A qualitative study. International Journal of Social Psychology, 62(7), 635-644

Quantitative Study:

Boyers, G. B., & Rowe, L. S. (2018). Social support and relationship satisfaction in bipolar disorder. Journal of Family Psychology, 32(4), 538-543.

Characteristic

Qualitative

Quantitative

Research design

Sample size

Sampling method

Procedure

Measures and instruments

Data analysis technique

Results

Readability of report

Ethical considerations

Any other thoughts?

1/30/2020 https://ashford.waypointoutcomes.com/assessment/23624/preview

https://ashford.waypointoutcomes.com/assessment/23624/preview 1/1

Description:

Total Possible Score: 3.00

Distinguished – A completed method comparison journal exercise table was submitted. All or most of the cells in the table contain
observations about the similarities and differences between the two research studies.

Proficient – N/A

Basic – N/A

Below Expectations – N/A

Non-Performance – The method comparison journal exercise is either nonexistent or lacks the components described in the
assignment instructions.

PSY326.W2J1.08.23.18

Completes the Method Comparison Journal Exercise Table Total: 3.00

Powered by

International Journal of
Social Psychiatry
2016, Vol. 62(7) 635 –644
© The Author(s) 2016
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0020764016666572
isp.sagepub.com

E CAMDEN SCHIZOPH

Introduction

Bipolar disorder (BD) is a chronic mental illness associ-
ated with reduced quality of life, decreased functioning,
high rates of suicide and high financial costs (Murray &
Lopez, 1997; Zaretsky, Rizvi, & Parikh, 2007). Prevalence
in the United States may be as high as 3.7% for BD spec-
trum disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2002;
Hirschfeld, Calabrese, & Weissman, 2002). A cornerstone
of treatment for individuals with BD is mood stabilizing
medications such as lithium, anticonvulsants and atypical
antipsychotic medication (American Psychiatric
Association, 2002; Goodwin & Young, 2003; Yatham
et al., 2005); yet, roughly half of individuals with BD are
non-adherent with medication (Lingam & Scott, 2002;
Perlick, Rosenheck, Kaczynski, & Kozma, 2004; Sajatovic,
Valenstein, Blow, Ganoczy, & Ignacio. 2006, 2007).
Concurrent with the use of medications, Chronic Disease
Self-Management (CDSM) programs can empower
patients and improve health outcomes by emphasizing the
central role of the individual in managing their mental and
physical health while collaborating with health-care pro-
fessionals and systems (Janney, Bauer, & Kilbourne, 2014;
Lorig, 2015; Lorig, Ritter, Pifer, & Werner, 2015).
Evidence-based skills shown to be effective in BD and
amenable to chronic disease self-management include the
following: psychoeducation, monitoring moods, social

functioning, sleep hygiene, setting goals and relapse plans
and adopting healthy lifestyle plans (Janney, Bauer, &
Kilbourne, 2014). However, self-management is challeng-
ing for many individuals with BD; there are numerous bar-
riers that can impede progress and success.

Few studies have specifically addressed patients’ per-
ceptions of barriers to self-management of BD; most have
focused on risk factors for poor adherence (Lingam &
Scott, 2002; Perlick et al., 2004; Sajatovic et al., 2006,
2007). In this qualitative analysis, perceived barriers to

Patients’ perceptions of barriers to
self-managing bipolar disorder:
A qualitative study

Carol Blixen1,2, Adam T Perzynski2, Ashley Bukach1,
Molly Howland3 and Martha Sajatovic4,5

Abstract
Background: Self-management of bipolar disorder (BD) is challenging for many individuals.
Material: Interviews were used to assess perceived barriers to disease self-management among 21 high-risk patients
with BD. Content analysis, with an emphasis on dominant themes, was used to analyze the data.
Results: Three major domains of barriers emerged: individual barriers (psychological, knowledge, behavioral and physical
health); family/community-level barriers (lack of support and resources); and provider/healthcare system (inadequate
communication and access to care).
Conclusion: Care approaches providing social and peer support, optimizing communication with providers and
integrating medical and psychiatric care may improve self-management of BD in this vulnerable population.

Keywords
Bipolar disorder, barriers, self-management

1 Department of Psychiatry, Case Western Reserve University,
Cleveland, OH, USA

2 Center for Health Care Research and Policy, MetroHealth Medical
Center, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH, USA

3 School of Medicine, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH,
USA

4 Department of Psychiatry, Neurology, and Biostatistics &
Epidemiology, School of Medicine, Case Western Reserve University,
Cleveland, OH, USA

5 Neurological Institute, University Hospitals Case Medical Center,
Cleveland, OH, USA

Corresponding author:
Carol Blixen, Department of Psychiatry, Case Western Reserve
University, Cleveland, OH 44106, USA.
Email: cxb28@cwru.edu

666572 ISP0010.1177/0020764016666572International Journal of Social PsychiatryBlixen et al.
research-article2016

Original Article

mailto:cxb28@cwru.edu

http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1177%2F0020764016666572&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2016-09-19

636 International Journal of Social Psychiatry 62(7)

self-management among high-risk patients with BD were
assessed as part of a large, on-going US National Institutes
of Mental Health (NIMH)-funded, randomized controlled
trial (RCT). This RCT is testing a novel customized adher-
ence enhancement (CAE) intervention intended to pro-
mote BD medication adherence versus an educational
control (EDU) intervention in poorly adherent individuals
with BD. Our findings can enrich our understanding of the
processes that impact the outcomes of this RCT and may
help clinicians and researchers integrate the consideration
of these factors into effective care delivery practices.

Methods

Sample and setting

Participants (n = 21) from the RCT were recruited at base-
line for the present analysis. For qualitative research, this
sample size is within the recommended number of 5–25
individuals who have all experienced the same phenomena
(Polkinghorne, 1989). Non-adherence was assessed by the
Tablets Routine Questionnaire (TRQ) as the percentage of
days with missed doses in the past week for each pre-
scribed foundational oral medication for the treatment of
BD. For individuals who were on one or more founda-
tional medication, an average was calculated in order to
gather information on the full BD treatment regimen.
Higher TRQ scores are a reflection of worse medication
adherence (Scott & Pope, 2002). In addition, a sampling
grid designed to ensure variability in gender, age, race/eth-
nicity and randomization group was used in the recruit-
ment of the medication non-adherent participants. The
study was conducted in the Department of Psychiatry of a
Midwestern urban hospital in the United States and was
approved by the local Institutional Review Board. All par-
ticipants provided written informed consent.

The mean age of the sample was 47.29 (standard devia-
tion (SD) = 11.06) years, 15 (71.4%) were women and the
mean level of education was 12.10 (SD = 2.31) years. Only
3 (14.3%) were married, and 18 (85.7%) were disabled or
unemployed. In all, 13 (61.9%), identified themselves as
African-American, 5 (23.8%) as Caucasian, 1 (4.8%) as
Hispanic and 3 (14.3%) as others. In all, 17 (81.0%) had
type I BD, and 3 (14.3%) had type II BD. The average age
of onset was 22.05 (SD = 10.31) years, and the average
number of psychiatric hospitalizations was 4.15
(SD = 3.47). We have limited data on comorbid physical
health conditions for the 21 participants in this qualitative
study. This type of information was not collected until
approximately 6 months after the study had started, when
many of them had completed their participation. However,
comorbidities reported for the whole sample in the RCT
(n = 160) revealed the leading conditions to be hyperten-
sion 59 (45.7%), arthritis 73 (45.6%) and high cholesterol
39 (38.0%).

Study design

In this cross-sectional qualitative study, a thematic analyti-
cal approach was used to develop a deeper understanding
of the perceived barriers to optimal self-management of
BD (Strauss, 1987). In this approach, researchers move
their analysis from a broad reading of the data toward dis-
covering patterns and developing themes.

Qualitative data collection and analysis

Individual face-to-face semi-structured interviews, con-
ducted as part of the baseline assessment in the RCT, were
used to collect narrative data on self-management barriers.
The goal of using this type of interview was to explore a
topic more openly and to allow interviewees to express
their opinions and ideas in their own words. Semi-
structured interviews are an appropriate strategy for learn-
ing the vocabulary, and discovering the thinking patterns,
of the target audience as well as for discovering unantici-
pated findings and exploring hidden meanings (Marshall
& Rossman, 2006). Therefore, respondents were given as
much latitude as possible to describe the strategies they
used to manage their chronic disease. A topic guide was
used to focus the discussion on main topics and specific
topic-related questions. For example, under the topic, ‘bar-
riers to managing BD’, the following question was asked:
‘What sort of things get in the way, or prevent you from
managing/taking care of your BD?’ Follow-up questions
such as ‘Would you explain further’, and ‘Would you give
me an example?’ were used to facilitate respondent com-
munication. Interviews, which lasted approximately an
hour, were audiotaped and transcribed verbatim.

In qualitative research, data collection, coding and anal-
ysis occur simultaneously. Emerging insights can be incor-
porated into later stages of data generation, enhancing the
comprehensiveness of the results (Strauss, 1987). We used
a thematic content analysis approach to data analysis,
encompassing open, axial and sequential coding and the
constant comparative method to generate constructs
(themes) and elaborate the relationship among constructs
(Strauss, 1987). A coding dictionary that included mutu-
ally exclusive code definitions was then constructed.
Coding structure was reviewed after a preliminary analysis
of a sub-sample of transcripts, and the dictionary was
refined through comparison, categorization and discussion
of each code’s properties and dimensions (Strauss, 1987).
Significant statements and themes attached to the codes
enabled identification/characterization of perceived barri-
ers to self-management of BD. Reduction of data in this
manner enabled us to write a composite description that
represented the essence of the phenomenon (perceptions
of ‘how’ and ‘why’) individuals with BD have problems
with self-managing. To ensure qualitative rigor throughout
the inquiry process, an audit trail was developed which

Blixen et al. 637

documented all research discussions, meetings and activi-
ties. In addition, two qualitatively trained investigators
(C.B., A.P.) independently coded each transcript to ensure
consistency and transparency of the coding; discrepancies
were resolved by discussion.

Results

Analysis of the data generated three major domains of bar-
riers to disease self-management among patients with BD:
(1) personal-level barriers, (2) family- and community-
level barriers and (3) provider- and health-care system–
level barriers.

Personal-level barriers

In Table 1, themes and illustrative quotations emerging
from the discussion of individual-level barriers to self-
management are shown. We classified these themes into
the four categories that reflected the personal barriers that
our respondents faced in trying to self-manage their BD:
(1) psychological, (2) knowledge, (3) behavioral and (4)
physical health.

Psychological barriers

Stigma and isolation. As noted in Table 1, having BD was
stigmatizing, causing respondents to feel perceived as dif-
ferent from others, resulting in a loss of self and social
isolation:

I feel different from other people. Sometime I feel God gave
me a bad hand. If I can’t think like most people, or you
know, do stuff like other people, it gets to me. (Respondent
#2004)

I like to play chess, but I don’t go nowhere to play chess.
Normally, I sit with the pieces by myself. (Respondent #2012)

Taking medications for BD was also perceived as stigma-
tizing because everyone would then know they had a men-
tal illness:

It took a long time for me to take the medicine because I
didn’t want to be classified as having a mental illness because
I thought I’d be ostracized … If my friends knew that I was
taking medicine because I was bipolar, they’ll say I am crazy.
(Respondent #2006)

Knowledge barriers

Diagnosis and causes. There was a paucity of knowledge
about BD, ranging from the diagnosis itself to causes of
the disease:

I had a long time problem trying to understand it because I
thought bipolar was two people in one and this evil side and
this good side would come in and out. (Respondent #2006)

… I still don’t understand what constitutes it. To understand it
is the first issue. And since I don’t understand what symptoms
are, I gotta first know ‘em before I can say I’m aware of ‘em.
(Respondent #2052)

While some described the cause of their BD as a ‘chemical
imbalance of the brain’ or ‘genetic’, many other causes
were cited which ranged from traumatic childhoods:

I’ve had a few tragedies, you know, coming up in childhood
and stuff, but I don’t know if that had something to do with it.
(Respondent #2009)

To being born of alcoholic parents as well as their own
alcohol and drug use:

I thought because me being a child alcoholic that has
something to do with my parents drinking when I was born.
(Respondent #2006)

Table 1. Personal-level barriers to disease self-management among poorly adherent patients with bipolar disorder (BD) (n = 21).

Themes and categories Illustrative quotations from respondents

Psychological barriers
Stigma and isolation

‘It’s kinda weird. People look at you differently. When you say, well, I have bipolar disorder, it’s like
you’re crazy or something. Stay away from her’. (Respondent #2015)
‘… I isolate a lot. I try to stay away from people; basically just stay in my room. I just lock my doors
and stay in the house all the time’. (Respondent #2052)

Knowledge barriers
Diagnosis and causes

‘I have no understanding of Bipolar Disorder. Once I understand what it is that I have and why
I’m what I am, then maybe I’ll understand why I sometimes respond or do things the way I do’.
(Respondent #2052)
‘I would like to know what causes it, but I don’t. I just figured I was just born like this’. (Respondent
#2012)

Behavioral barriers

Attitudes and lifestyle
issues

‘I don’t like being controlled by my medication, and being so dependent upon it. I just try to do it on
my own. I don’t want to be stuck on medicine all the rest of my life’. (Respondent #2009)
‘Bipolar is a serious disease because I make irrational, crazy decisions, mainly when I’m manic. Then I
have to face the consequences’. (Respondent #2003)

Physical health barriers ‘But being bipolar and dealing with having HIV is sort of like a tough issue because I’m dealing with
two things!’ (Respondent #2005)

638 International Journal of Social Psychiatry 62(7)

It has something to do with my brain, you know. I know I did
a lot of damage to myself. … drugs and alcohol play a part in
damaging my brain. (Respondent #2005)

Most respondents described their symptoms of BD as
mood swings, manic behavior and/or depression, but one
respondent described his symptoms as something called
the HALT:

But then when I get home, sometime I get real lonely and get
that H-A-L- T–where you get hungry, angry, lonely and tired.
… it’s like I’m shutting down, and I don’t know when I’m
coming out of it. (Respondent #2015)

Behavioral barriers

Attitudes. All the respondents agreed that BD was a very
serious illness:

It’s basically one of those illnesses, like cancer or AIDS or
something like that. So, it’s very serious, like heart attacks or
those illnesses that can take your life. (Respondent #2020)

However, negative or ambivalent attitudes about medica-
tions prescribed for their BD posed barriers to self-man-
agement. These included complaints about side effects,
which ranged from dry mouth, inability to concentrate,
drowsiness, weight gain to out-of-body experiences:

… it’s almost like an out-of-body experience. Like even just
looking into the world, like walking down the street, it would
seem so far away. And it would seem like it took extra effort
to pick up my legs and to move … it was a very uncomfortable
and edgy feeling. And I couldn’t handle it. (Respondent
#2016)

Frustration with keeping up a medication routine and skip-
ping medications on days when they were feeling good
were also common attitudes:

A lot of times I stop taking my medication, because I get tired
of just the routine of taking medication. I’ll just get up one
morning and just say ‘I ain’t taking it’. Then a few days go by
where I haven’t took it and then I just start lookin for some
drugs. (Respondent #2012)

Sometimes you feel you don’t need to take it maybe because
you feel fine or something, so the good days could be skip
days. (Respondent #2005)

For some, being in a relationship meant that they avoided
taking medications, while others felt that they didn’t need
to take them:

If I start a relationship, I stop taking medication because I
don’t want them to know what I got (BD). (Respondent #2007)

I don’t see myself being one of those people who need it
(medication). So, I have a messed up way of thinking.
(Respondent #2020)

Lifestyle issues. One of the major barriers to self-manage-
ment was making irrational decisions which led to a spiral
of negative consequences and despair:

I make irrational decisions mainly when I’m manic. I make
crazy, bad decisions, and then I have consequences. I got
arrested because I had warrants on me from 2012 for being
argumentative and irritable in public places. And when the
cops approached me, I would be yelling and screaming. And
then I got jail for eight days and lost my job … (Respondent
#2013)

One respondent described her sudden decision to move out
of state with her three children because she looked on-line
and saw

… these big beautiful houses they have in Georgia, that you
can get for less price that you can get here. The problem was
I didn’t have enough money and no support. I didn’t have a
car and everybody would tell me not to go to Georgia without
having transportation. But I went anyway, sold everything
and dragged my three kids with me, uprooted them. I was
manic, I was excited and then I came back to reality, and
moved back here, and became very depressed after that.
(Respondent #2015)

Limited finances, lack of transportation and drug and alcohol
use were also cited as barriers to self-management of BD:

I have no social activities. I have no money to socialize, even
Bingo costs money. (Respondent #2002)

I have a lot of problems going to get meds and seeing my
doctor. A lot of times I don’t have bus fare. (Respondent
#2012)

I’m a recovering addict, and I sometimes have my back slides.
I’m somebody who experimented with a lot of things and just
got it down to just two, smoking marijuana or doing cocaine
or crack. I’m weaning myself – slowly but surely. (Respondent
#205)

Physical health barriers

As noted in Table 1 and below, respondents also cited
comorbid physical illnesses as barriers to management of
their mental illness:

… I have diabetes, chronic arthritis, and chronic asthma. I
have a history of pulmonary embolisms and deep vein
thrombosis. Did I miss any? Oh, I also have high blood
pressure and asthma. (Respondent #2093)

Blixen et al. 639

I was diagnosed with diabetes type one thirteen years ago. I
take insulin and that interacts with bipolar and causes mood
swings too. It’s even more dangerous to have type one
interacting with bipolar than type two because you go from
low to high very quickly. (Respondent #2013)

In summary, respondents in the study cited many personal
barriers to self-management of BD which included per-
ceived stigma and isolation, lack of knowledge about the
disease itself and their own negative attitudes toward med-
ications which ranged from denial of their need for them to
feelings of being controlled and dependent. Other personal
barriers included their own chaotic lifestyles and comorbid
physical diseases which also complicated management of
their BD.

Family- and community-level barriers

Table 2 shows themes, descriptive codes and illustrative
quotations emerging from the discussion of family- and
community-level barriers. Two key barrier categories were
as follows: (1) limited understanding of BD and (2) limited
community resources.

Limited understanding of BD

Lack of support. In addition to their feeling isolated and
alone, respondents felt an overwhelming lack of sup-
port from family and the community in which they
lived:

Well, they try, but they don’t really know. They think they
know and a lot of them just don’t think there’s anything wrong
with me. Like the lady I stay with, she try to understand, but
she really don’t understand. (Respondent #2012)

Well my husband, he has a mental illness too. Schizophrenic
… paranoia. I mean, he might understand but he basically
don’t like to talk about certain things anyways, so I don’t
know if he understands. (Respondent #2005)

Estrangement. Having BD sometimes led to estrangement
from family:

Yep, that’s what my family does, they avoid me. And they say
it’s because I live on the west side, and they live all the way
out on the east side. They got a car. I don’t. I compare my
situation to my friends and their relationship with their
families and I’m like well, her cousins come over and visit
her. You know, it’s like no matter where I live if you’re
concerned, then show it! (Respondent #2007)

I don’t have family in the United States. It’s just me. The last
few years really, my communications with them went from
bad to worse. (Respondent #2013)

Stressful relationships. Friction, misunderstandings and
sometimes abuse added additional barriers to self-manag-
ing their BD:

And like even with your family, you can tend to get more
stressed, get angry at each other and get frustrated …
(Respondent #2005)

I live with my boyfriend and it’s been an on and off twenty
year abusive relationship. And as my mind gets a little bit
clearer, he’s not liking it. He’s so used to keeping me, like in
his control, you know. He’s been physical with me and with
my dog, cause when he goes at me, my dog goes at him. And
he don’t like that. I’ve put him in prison before for domestic
violence. (Respondent #2016)

Negative attitudes about medications for BD

Because of misinformation and beliefs about BD, family
members and even community support groups often gave
respondents incorrect advice about taking medications for
their BD:

My family is always telling me ‘I don’t think you need to take
the medication, I think you need to call the doctor’.
(Respondent #2011)

… and people telling me ‘Girl you don’t need that medicine,
just all you need to do is cut the stress in your life, you don’t
need the medicine. You looked zooted out. You know, I can
tell you’re on medicine’. And that makes me say ‘Okay, I
don’t need it no more’. But I know I do. (Respondent #2007)

Table 2. Family and community barriers to disease self-management among poorly adherent patients with bipolar disorder (BD)
(n = 21).

Themes and categories Illustrative quotations from respondents

Limited understanding
of BD

‘My father thinks I should just get over it, that it’s just in my head. Like I’m making it out to be more
than what it is. My father just don’t get it and I don’t think he ever will. He’s just set in his ways and
he thinks I’m just stupid and just do these things. (Respondent #2016)
‘You have people that tell you, “Oh you don’t need that (medications), you just need God.” But you
know, I know I need them. I know that I need it, that’s the one thing I know’. (Respondent #2008)

Limited community
resources

‘Right now I stay with a friend. I’m homeless. I been tryin to get some help with subsidized housing,
but I can’t seem to get no help, unless I stay at this shelter, S.G. And I know if I go down to S.G. and
stay, I’ll wind up usin drugs, cause it’s a lot of drugs down there’. (Respondent #2012)

640 International Journal of Social Psychiatry 62(7)

I used to go to this one AA meeting, and they were saying no
drugs or alcohol whatsoever and they tried to implicate that
you shouldn’t even take mental health drug and stuff because
they felt that the effect of them got you high, which is not true.
(Respondent #2006)

Limited community resources

Unstable living situations, homelessness and limited
income often led to living in shelters where self-managing
BD was especially difficult and led to a spiral of despair:

I stay with a friend, but she don’t want me there, because she
wants an intimate relationship. And I don’t want an intimate
relationship with her. So, basically, I don’t have a home.
(Respondent #2012)

The problem is a lot of housing programs I do not qualify for,
because I’m not a permanent resident or an American citizen
for the last seven years, which is a requirement. So I have to
go back down to the shelter. It’s been very hard for me to
accept it. And that’s triggering my depression. (Respondent
#2013)

When you have a limited income, it’s hard to find housing.
It’s hard to find programs where you can go to be able to get
some type of help. They don’t give you any avenues of
resources. They’ll tell you ‘well, go down to the welfare
building’. Welfare building cannot help you … it’s even more
frustrating when you get down there and you have to deal
with the social workers, supervisors, the people down there,
the people waiting in the lobby. It’s crazy and it’s another
thing, more stress added on to you. (Respondent #2011)

In summary, family and community issues such as lack of
social support, limited understanding of BD, misinforma-
tion about medication, stressful relationships and limited
income and housing options posed what respondents per-
ceived as insurmountable barriers to self-management of
BD.

Provider- and health-care system–level barriers

Table 3 shows themes, descriptive codes and illustrative
quotations emerging from the discussion of provider- and
health-care system–level barriers. Two key categories of
barriers emerged from the data: (1) patient/provider rela-
tionships and (2) access to care.

Patient/provider relationships

Ineffective communication. For respondents, the major bar-
rier to having a good patient/provider relationship was the
inability of their provider to communicate with them.
Many times respondents didn’t understand what their pro-
viders told them during visits because of the use of unfa-
miliar words or medical jargon:

I’m developmentally delayed, and I don’t comprehend things
as well as most people. If you don’t break it down to something
in basically layman’s terms that I can understand, I’m not
gonna understand what they say … (Respondent #2020)

The emphasis on medication-prescribing during the visit,
instead of listening, was also seen as a barrier to effective
communication:

I’m looking at her and she’s looking at me and it’s making me
feel like I’m really crazy or I don’t know what I’m saying. It’s
hard to get them to understand and they just give you you
pills, and bye bye! I don’t want your drugs. I just want you to
hear me, what I’m telling you! (Respondent #2004)

Another reported aspect of poor provider communication
skills was provider body language that made them feel as
though they were ‘just another patient’:

She really didn’t say much. She kinda just sat there… no
rapport whatsoever. It wasn’t that I didn’t understand what
she was saying, but her body language! A lot of times people
don’t realize your body language will speak louder than what
your verbal words say. When I see that your body language is
speaking to me, as if you’re like, ‘oK’ here we go with another
hard luck person with a hundred problems. (Respondent
#2011)

Access to care

Appointment issues. As noted in Table 3, getting an appoint-
ment with a mental health provider was a long and frustrat-
ing process, and infrequent scheduled appointments left
too much time for respondents to remember what they
wanted to report or talk about:

I’ve seen him maybe twice over the last six months. By the
time I get around to seeing him, I can’t remember. But I know
there’s something important I want to talk to him about, but
we’re already on another issue, then I can’t frame what the
question’s gonna be or why I’m asking the question.
(Respondent #2052)

Turnover in providers. Most of the respondents received
their care from resident physician trainees who often
rotated off the service after a period of time. Turnover in
providers left respondents feeling frustrated and often pre-
vented the formation of a therapeutic relationship. For
those who had made progress in managing their BD, the
loss of the provider was especially discouraging:

… at L.S. you never can tell who you’re gonna get. It’s like a
box of chocolates … I could have somebody one week and
somebody the next week. … I always want the same person.
I’ll wait three months to get an appointment, and then that
person is no longer there. So, there’s a turnover. And I don’t
get to build up a relationship. (Respondent #2093)

Blixen et al. 641

Well I wish she had stayed. She left there and went over to B.
She was, as far as I’m concerned, the very best therapist that I
had. I was making leaps and bounds with her and being able
to be myself, which is rare. I have a new one. His name’s Q.I.
They canceled the last meeting. I’m tryin to get back on track
to where I see him. (Respondent #2052)

In summary, respondents cited ineffective communication
with their provider as a major barrier to forming a thera-
peutic relationship. Communication problems were com-
pounded by difficulties getting an appointment and the
subsequent rapid turnover in providers.

Discussion

In this well-characterized sample of poorly adherent peo-
ple with BD, qualitative analysis identified a number of
barriers to self-management that have important implica-
tions as to how best to help and empower high-risk indi-
viduals. First, participants with BD identified barriers to
self-management that spanned individual, family/commu-
nity and provider/healthcare system domains. This is con-
sistent with McLeroy, Bibeau, Steckler, and Glanz’s (1988)
social ecological model of health behavior. This model
posits that behavior, actions and events are influenced by
individual, interpersonal, organizational, community and
policy factors.

Second, the internalized stigma associated with having
a mental illness was clearly articulated by some partici-
pants. Internalized stigma, or self-stigma, occurs when
individuals accept society’s assessment and incorporate it
into their sense of self (Corrigan, Kerr, & Knudsen, 2005;
Ellison, Mason, & Scior, 2013; Latalova et al., 2013). In a
meta-analysis of internalized stigma in people living with
mental illness, Livingston and Boyd (2010) found a

striking negative relationship between internalized stigma
and psychosocial variables (hope, self-esteem and empow-
erment), medication adherence and a positive relationship
with psychiatric symptom severity. In addition to internal-
ized stigma, or perhaps because of it, isolation and a loss of
self were common themes among participants. Charmaz
(1983) describes this loss of self as a form of suffering felt
by those living with a chronic illness who ‘observe their
former self-images crumbling away without the simultane-
ous development of equally valued ones’. This author con-
cludes that this loss of self results in ‘restricted lives, social
isolation, and feeling that one’s illness has become a major
source of identity’.

Third, while social support is known to be helpful for
chronic illness self-management (Gallant, 2003; Strom &
Egede, 2012), in this study, there appeared to be a marked
absence of social support from families and communities.
This included negative attitudes toward psychotropic med-
ications and limited resources such as income and housing
resources. Medication adherence in BD treatment has been
shown to be related to a number of factors, among them
psychosocial support, number of comorbid illnesses and
attitudes toward medications (Lingam & Scott, 2002;
Perlick et al., 2004; Sajatovic et al., 2007).

Fourth, communication is crucial to building a thera-
peutic clinician–patient relationship and delivering high-
quality care (Arora, 2003; Fong Ha & Longnecker, 2010;
Roter, 1983; Stewart, 1995); yet, there were those respond-
ents who felt that they couldn’t understand what their pro-
vider said, or they were viewed impersonally, ‘as just
another patient’. Ineffective provider communication and
resultant dissatisfaction are not unique to the participants
in our study, and patient surveys have consistently shown
a desire for better communication with their clinicians
(Duffy et al., 2004).

Table 3. Provider and health-care system–level barriers to disease self-management among poorly adherent patients with bipolar
disorder (n = 21).

Themes and categories Illustrative quotations from respondents

Patient/provider
relationships

‘That’s fine and well, but it would be nice if I understood what is all goin on, so I can at least
appreciate the visit, half hour though it may be … I: … then I can at least understand or know which
direction I’m goin, or at least I’ll have a workin knowledge of why you’re asking the questions you
are asking me, or whatever. When I see my therapist again, I’m gonna ask him to explain to me what
the hell bipolar is – nobody ever told me’. (Respondent #2052)
‘The same questions every time. It’s just a standard group of questions. Do I hear voices? Can I read
other people’s minds? Sometime I want to tell what’s goin on. Like, if my brother, sister dies, my
past. I’ll get upset about that and I want to vent. I wanna get that out. It’s like, I go in just to get a
prescription for meds’. (Respondent #2093)

Access to care ‘It’s been over a year. They state you have to have a referral. Well, if that’s the case I’m still waiting
almost two years for that same referral for someone to call me. Hey look, what does it take? Me to
hurt myself or someone else or to really have a bad, you know, reaction for myself, in order to get
some help. I need to speak to somebody. I got a lot of issues’. (Respondent #2089)
‘… but every six months they give you a new one (therapist) anyway. So you don’t really have a
chance to have a close relationship. So, personally I try not to discuss anything with them that I feel is
very personal to me’. (Respondent #2006)

642 International Journal of Social Psychiatry 62(7)

An additional finding from this qualitative analysis was
that reported comorbid physical illnesses complicated self-
management of BD. Physical illnesses are more prevalent
in people with serious mental illness (SMI) than in the
general population (DeHertet al., 2011), but there is a sig-
nificant lack of awareness of the physical health and
health-care access problems for people with SMI. Mental
health treatments in the United States are often delivered
separately from clinical settings for primary, or other,
medical specialty care. Most of the respondents in this
study received their psychiatric care from Community
Mental Health Centers (CMHCs) which generally provide
an array of mental health services; however, care is often
not integrated with primary care or other specialty care. In
addition, medical settings may be poorly equipped to
assess and manage individuals with more severe psychiat-
ric symptoms. A recent systematic review of mortality in
mental disorders (Walker, McGhee, & Druss, 2015) noted
that approximately two-thirds of deaths in people with
mental disorders are due to causes like heart disease and
diabetes. Care of chronic medical conditions in those with
mental illness requires an approach that promotes healthy
behaviors and coordinates care between mental health and
medical systems.

It is possible that changing elements in the health-care
climate, such as the addition of primary care services to
CMHC infrastructures, increased use of electronic health
records that facilitate communication between primary and
specialty care providers, and between providers and
patients, may help to minimize the barriers to physical and
mental health–integrated care delivery. Furthermore, many
experts believe that the increased presence of medical
homes, a model that seeks to facilitate partnerships among
the patient, his or her primary care physician and the
patient’s family can solve many of the problems related to
concurrent care of people with both physical and mental
illnesses (Bodenheimer & Pham, 2010). Another element
to incorporate into the health-care system may be the use of
peer educators. Peer support, provided by individuals who
have a SMI such as BD, can teach and model self-manage-
ment, help normalize the illness experience, promote hope
for recovery and increase feelings of empowerment and
self-esteem (Repper & Carter, 2011; Schon, 2015). Peers
may also help in reducing the feelings of stigma associated
with BD, provide social support for individuals who feel
isolated and improve outcomes. Indeed, a recent evaluation
of peer support services for individuals with SMI (Chinman,
et al., 2014) found that compared with professional staff,
peers were better able to reduce inpatient use and improve
a range of recovery outcomes.

Although our findings on overall barriers to disease
self-management among patients with BD have implica-
tions for informing care, there are some limitations.
Patients with BD who receive care in other treatment set-
tings, or those who have less severe or disabling illness,

may have different experiences in managing this chronic
illness and different types of encounters with providers
and health-care systems. At the same time, the focus on
poorly adherent patients facilitated an understanding on
barriers to self-management among those who are most in
need for intervention. The small convenience sample and
the conduct of the study in a single urban area in the United
States may limit transferability of the study findings. In
addition, our inability to obtain only limited information
on comorbid physical health conditions for our 21 partici-
pants may indicate that this randomly chosen sample may
not be representative of the entire data set. These limita-
tions are offset, to some extent, by the use of rigorous
qualitative research methods described in this study . The
self-report method is direct, versatile and yields informa-
tion that would be difficult, if not impossible, to gather by
other means.

Conclusion

Our findings indicate that poorly adherent patients with
BD had internalized the sense of stigma associated with
having a mental illness. Additionally, they had inadequate
knowledge about the causes and management of their
mental disorder, little or no social support from family and
community, stressful relationships, family estrangement,
multiple comorbid conditions, alcohol and substance
abuse, and chaotic lifestyles. All these factors posed innu-
merable barriers to self-management for the study respond-
ents. Care approaches that provide social and peer support,
locate resources, optimize communication with providers
and integrate medical and psychiatric care may improve
self-management and reduce health complications in this
vulnerable population.

Additional areas of future research should further
explore the topic of adherence enhancement in individuals
with BD as it relates to specific barriers. Our qualitative
data illustrate that adherence barriers are not uniform, and
it is likely that a one-size-fits-all approach will not satisfy
the needs of many with this chronic mental illness.
Targeted, or personalized, approaches that address specific
challenges to adherence are likely to yield benefits that can
help diverse individuals in a variety of settings.

Conflict of interest

The author(s) declared the following potential conflicts of inter-
est with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of
this article: Dr Sajatovic has research grants from Pfizer, Merck,
Janssen, Reuter Foundation, Woodruff Foundation, Reinberger
Foundation, National Institute of Health (NIH) and the Centers
for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). Dr Sajatovic is a con-
sultant to Bracket, Prophase, Otsuka, Pfizer and Sunovion and
has received royalties from Springer Press, Johns Hopkins
University Press, Oxford Press, UpToDate and Lexicomp. None
of the other authors declare conflict of interest.

Blixen et al. 643

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support
for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article:
Research reported in this publication was supported by the
National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of
Health under award no. R01MH093321. Support was also
received from the Clinical and Translational Science of
Cleveland, UL1TR000439, from the National Center for
Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS) component of the
National Institutes of Health and NIH Roadmap for Medical
Research. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors
and does not necessarily represent the official views of the
National Institutes of Health.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2002). Practice guideline for
the treatment of patients with bipolar disorder (revision).
The American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 1–50.

Arora, N. (2003). Interacting with cancer patients: The sig-
nificance of physicians’ communication behavior. Social
Science & Medicine, 57, 791–806.

Bodenheimer, T., & Pham, H. H. (2010). Primary care: Current
problems and proposed solutions. Health Affairs, 29, 799–
805.

Charmaz, K. (1983). Loss of self: A fundamental form of suffer-
ing in the chronically ill. Sociology of Health and Illness,
5, 168–195.

Chinman, M., Preethy, G., Dougherty, R., Daniels, A., Ghose, S.,
Swift, A., & Delphin-Rittmon, M. (2014). Peer support ser-
vices for individuals with serious mental illness: Assessing
the evidence. Psychiatric Services, 65, 429–441.

Corrigan, P. W., Kerr, A., & Knudsen, L. (2005). The stigma of
mental illness: Explanatory models and methods for change.
Applied and Preventive Psychology, 11, 179–190.

DeHert, M., Correl, C. U., Bobes J., Cetkovich-Bakmas, M.,
Cohen, D., Asai, I., & Leucht, S. (2011). Physical illness
in patients with severe mental disorders. I. Prevalence,
impact of medications and disparities in health care. World
Psychiatry, 10, 52–77.

Duffy, F. D., Gordon, G. H., Whelan, G., Cole-Kelly, K.,
Frankel, R., Buffone, N., & Langdon, L. (2004). Assessing
competence in communication and interpersonal skills: The
Kalamazoo II report. Academic Medicine, 79, 495–507.

Ellison, N., Mason, O., & Scior, K. (2013). Bipolar disorder and
stigma: A systematic review of the literature. Journal of
Affective Disorders, 151, 805–820.

Fong Ha, J., & Longnecker, N. (2010). Doctor-patient communi-
cation: A review. The Ochsner Journal, 10, 38–43.

Gallant, M. (2003). The influence of social support on chronic
illness self-management: A review and directions for
research. Health Education & Behavior, 30, 170–195.

Goodwin, G. M., & Young, A. H. (2003). The British association for
psychopharmacology guidelines for treatment of bipolar dis-
order: A summary. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 17, 3–6.

Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Calabrese, J. R., & Weissman, M. (2002,
May 18–23) Lifetime prevalence of bipolar I and II dis-
orders in the United States. Paper Presented at 155th
Annual Meeting of the American Psychiatric Association,
Philadelphia, PA.

Janney, C., Bauer, M., & Kilbourne, A. (2014). Self-management
and bipolar disorder-A clinician’ guide to the literature 2011–
2014. Current Psychiatry Reports, 16, 485. doi:10.1007/
s11920-014-0485-5

Latalova, K., Ociskova, M., Prasko, J., Kamaradova, D.,
Jelenova, D., & Sedlackova, Z. (2013). Self-stigmatization
in patients with bipolar disorder. Neuro Endocrinology
Letters, 34, 265–272.

Lingam, R., & Scott, J. (2002). Treatment non-adherence in affec-
tive disorders. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 105, 164–172.

Livingston, J. D., & Boyd, J. E. (2010). Correlates and conse-
quences of internalized stigma for people living with men-
tal illness: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Social
Science & Medicine, 71, 2150–2161.

Lorig, K. (2015). Chronic disease self-management program:
Insights from the eye of the storm. Front Public Health,
253, 1–3. doi:10.2289/fpubh.2014.00253

Lorig, K., Ritter, P., Pifer, C., & Werner, P. (2015). Effectiveness
of the chronic disease self-management program for per-
sons with a serious mental illness: A translation study.
Community Mental Health Journal, 50, 96–103.

Marshall, C., & Rossman, G. (2006). Designing qualitative
research (4th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.

McLeroy, K., Bibeau, D., Steckler, A., & Glanz, K. (1988).
An ecological perspective on health promotion programs.
Health Education Quarterly, 15, 351–377.

Murray, C. J., & Lopez, A. D. (1997). Global mortality, disabil-
ity, and the contribution of risk factors: Global Burden of
Disease Study. The Lancet, 349, 1436–1442.

Perlick, D. A., Rosenheck, R. A., Kaczynski, R., & Kozma, L.
(2004). Medication non-adherence in bipolar disorder:
A patient-centered review of research findings. Clinical
Approaches in Bipolar Disorders, 3, 56–64.

Polkinghorne, D. E. (1989). Phenomenological research
methods. In R. S. Valle & S. Halling (Eds.), Existential-
phenomenological perspectives in psychology (pp. 41–60).
New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Repper, J., & Carter, T. (2011). A review of the literature on peer
support in mental health services. Journal of Mental Health,
20, 392–411.

Roter, D. L. (1983). Physician/patient communication:
Transmission of information and patient effects. Maryland
State Medical Journal, 32, 260–265.

Sajatovic, M., Valenstein, M., Blow, F. C., Ganocza D., & Ignacio,
R. (2006). Treatment adherence with antipsychotic medica-
tions in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 8, 232–241.

Sajatovic, M., Valenstein, M., Blow, F. C., Ganocza D., &
Ignacio, R. (2007). Treatment adherence with lithium and
anticonvulsant medications among patients with bipolar dis-
order. Psychiatric Services, 58, 855–863.

Schon, U. K. (2015). The power of identification: Peer support
in recovery from mental illness. Scandinavian Journal of
Disability Research, 12, 83–90.

Scott, J., & Pope, M. (2002). Self-reported adherence to treat-
ment with mood stabilizers, plasma levels, and psychiatric
hospitalizations. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 159,
1927–1929.

Stewart, M. A. (1995). Effective physician-patient communi-
cation and health outcomes: A review. Canadian Medical
Association Journal, 152, 1423–1433.

644 International Journal of Social Psychiatry 62(7)

Strauss, A. L. (1987). Qualitative analysis for social scientists.
New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Strom, J., & Egede, L. (2012). The impact of social sup-
port on outcomes in adult patients with type 2 diabe-
tes: A systematic review. Current Diabetes Reports, 2,
769–778.

Walker, E. R., McGhee, R. E., & Druss, B. G. (2015). Mortality
in mental disorders and global disease burden implications.
JAMA Psychiatry, 72, 334–341.

Yatham, L. N., Kennedy, S. H., O’Donovan, C., Parikh, S.,
MacQueen, G., McIntyre, R., & Gorman, C.P. (2005).
Canadian Network for Mood and Anxiety Treatments
(CANMAT) guidelines for the management of patients
with bipolar disorder: Consensus and controversies. Bipolar
Disorder, 7(Suppl. 3), 5–69.

Zaretsky, A. E., Rizvi, S., & Parikh, S. V. (2007). How well
do psychosocial interventions work in bipolar disorder?
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 52, 14–21.

BRIEF REPORT

  • Social Support and Relationship Satisfaction in Bipolar Disorder
  • Grace B. Boyers and Lorelei Simpson Rowe
    Southern Methodist University

    Social support is positively associated with individual well-being, particularly if an intimate partner provides
    that support. However, despite evidence that individuals with bipolar disorder (BPD) are at high risk for
    relationship discord and are especially vulnerable to low or inadequate social support, little research has
    explored the relationship between social support and relationship quality among couples in which a partner has
    BPD. The current study addresses this gap in the literature by examining the association between social
    support and relationship satisfaction in a weekly diary study. Thirty-eight opposite-sex couples who were
    married or living together for at least one year and in which one partner met diagnostic criteria for BPD
    completed up to 26 weekly diaries measuring social support and relationship satisfaction, as well as psychiatric
    symptoms. Results revealed that greater social support on average was associated with higher average
    relationship satisfaction for individuals with BPD and their partners, and that more support than usual in any
    given week was associated with higher relationship satisfaction that week. The converse was also true, with
    greater-than-average relationship satisfaction and more satisfaction than usual associated with greater social
    support. The results emphasize the week-to-week variability of social support and relationship satisfaction and
    the probable reciprocal relationship between support and satisfaction among couples in which a partner has
    BPD. Thus, social support may be important for maintaining relationship satisfaction and vice versa, even after
    controlling for concurrent mood symptoms.

    Keywords: bipolar disorder, marriage, social support, longitudinal, relationship satisfaction

    Bipolar disorder (BPD) is a severe and chronic illness charac-
    terized by extreme mood shifts (American Psychiatric Association,
    2000) and impairment in occupational and social functioning, even
    between affective episodes (Fagiolini et al., 2005; Judd & Akiskal,
    2003). Individuals with BPD are less likely to marry or live with
    a romantic partner, and those who do are at higher risk for
    relationship distress and dissolution compared to individuals with
    other psychiatric disorders and those without mental illness (Co-
    ryell et al., 1993; Judd & Akiskal, 2003; Whisman, 2007). Rela-
    tionship dysfunction has been attributed to a number of factors,
    including patient mood symptoms (e.g., Lam, Donaldson, Brown,
    & Malliaris, 2005), caregiver burden (Reinares et al., 2006), and
    deficits in psychosocial functioning (Coryell et al., 1993). Partners

    of individuals with BPD are also at risk for social, occupational,
    and financial distress, and symptoms of depression and anxiety
    (Lam et al., 2005). The high risk for individual and couple distress
    has led to calls to investigate factors that may buffer the negative
    effects of illness and improve functioning among individuals with
    BPD and their partners (Reinares et al., 2006).

    One potential buffering factor is social support. Multiple studies
    with nonclinical samples have demonstrated a positive association
    between social support and individual well-being (for a review, see
    Cohen & Wills, 1985), particularly when an intimate partner is the
    support provider (e.g., Beach, Martin, Blum, & Roman, 1993).
    This effect has been documented with both self-report and ob-
    served data, concurrently and over time (e.g., Cutrona & Suhr,
    1994; Sullivan, Pasch, Johnson, & Bradbury, 2010). Moreover,
    social support appears to buffer the effects of individual and
    couple-level stress on individual and relationship functioning
    (Bodenmann, 1995) and facilitate caring and intimacy (Cohen &
    Wills, 1985). This research is consistent with the intimacy process
    model (Reis & Patrick, 1996), which suggests that intimacy de-
    velops through exchanges that convey validation and understand-
    ing, especially in response to expressions of vulnerability. In
    contrast, inadequate or miscarried social support attempts are
    associated with declines in relationship quality over time (e.g.,
    Brock & Lawrence, 2009).

    For individuals with BPD, lack of social support (in either the
    quality or the number of supportive relationships) is associated
    with lower medication compliance and greater stress (e.g., Kul-
    hara, Basu, Mattoo, Sharan, & Chopra, 1999). In contrast, the

    Grace B. Boyers and Lorelei Simpson Rowe, Department of Psychology,
    Southern Methodist Universit

    y.

    The analyses presented in this study were conducted in fulfillment of
    Grace B. Boyers’s master’s thesis and have not previously been published.

    Previous versions of the analyses presented in this study were presented
    as a poster at the Annual Conference of the Association for Behavioral and
    Cognitive Therapies in November 2015 and as a paper at the Annual
    Conference of the Southwestern Psychological Association in April 2016.
    Other analyses using this data set were presented in Rowe and Miller
    Morris (2012).

    Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Lo-
    relei Simpson Rowe, Department of Psychology, Southern Methodist
    University, P.O. Box 750442, Dallas, TX 75275-0442. E-mail:
    lsimpson@smu.edu

    T
    hi

    s
    do

    cu
    m

    en
    t

    is
    co

    py
    ri

    gh
    te

    d
    by

    th
    e

    A
    m

    er
    ic

    an
    P

    sy
    ch

    ol
    og

    ic
    al

    A
    ss

    oc
    ia

    ti
    on

    or
    on

    e
    of

    it
    s

    al
    li

    ed
    pu

    bl
    is

    he
    rs

    .
    T

    hi
    s

    ar
    ti

    cl
    e

    is
    in

    te
    nd

    ed
    so

    le
    ly

    fo
    r

    th
    e

    pe
    rs

    on
    al

    us
    e

    of
    th

    e
    in

    di
    vi

    du
    al

    us
    er

    an
    d

    is
    no

    t
    to

    be
    di

    ss
    em

    in
    at

    ed
    br

    oa
    dl

    y.

    Journal of Family Psychology © 2018 American Psychological Association
    2018, Vol. 32, No. 4,

    538

    –543 0893-3200/18/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/fam0000400

    538

    mailto:lsimpson@smu.edu

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/fam0000400

    presence of support predicts longer time between recurrence of
    affective episodes (Cohen, Hammen, Henry, & Daley, 2004; John-
    son, Lundström, Åberg-Wistedt, & Mathé, 2003) and quicker
    recovery from mood episodes (Johnson, Winett, Meyer, Green-
    house, & Miller, 1999). However, no known research has directly
    studied the association between social support and relationship
    satisfaction within the context of BPD. This is particularly impor-
    tant because, although individuals with BPD have a high need for
    social support, they often do not receive it (Coryell et al., 1993).
    Likewise, their partners receive less social support than partners of
    individuals without mental illness; this has been attributed to
    limited social activities as well as lower support from the partner
    with BPD (Dore & Romans, 2001). In the current study, we
    examine the association between social support and relationship
    satisfaction among individuals with BPD and their intimate part-
    ners using an intensive longitudinal diary method. This method
    permits evaluation of fluctuation of variables over time, whereas
    the existing, predominantly cross-sectional research does not. That
    is, we can assess the overall association between relationship
    satisfaction and social support as well as the association between
    fluctuations in each variable.

    Second, we focus on each participant’s report of emotional
    support they received from their partner (e.g., expressions of care
    and understanding). We focus on perceived social support because
    associations between one partner’s report of support provision and
    the other’s report of support receipt are often weak (Haber, Cohen,
    Lucas, & Baltes, 2007), reflecting the subjective nature of social
    support and variability in support provision skill (Howland &
    Simpson, 2010). That is, one partner may engage in actions in-
    tended to be supportive that the other partner does not perceive as
    helpful, which can decrease relationship satisfaction (Bolger &
    Amarel, 2007). We also focus on emotional support, specifically,
    because it is more universally acceptable than instrumental support
    (i.e., active assistance; Cutrona & Suhr, 1992).

    We examined weekly reports of partner provision of social
    support from individuals with BPD and their partners, hypothesiz-
    ing that (a) individuals with BPD would report receiving more
    support than would their partners. We also tested the hypotheses
    that (b) support would be positively associated with relationship
    satisfaction on average and (c) support in any given week would be
    positively associated with relationship satisfaction in that week.
    Finally, because there is reason to believe that social support and
    relationship satisfaction build upon each other in a reciprocal
    fashion (Dunkel-Schetter & Skokan, 1990), we tested the converse
    hypotheses that (d) relationship satisfaction would be associated
    with support on average and (e) satisfaction in any given week
    would be positively associated with support in that week. We
    controlled for patient and partner depressive symptoms and patient
    manic symptoms because own and partner symptoms correlate
    with relationship satisfaction and social support (Lam et al., 2005;
    Lee et al., 2011; Whisman, Uebelacker, & Weinstock, 2004).

    Method

    Participants

    Thirty-eight individuals with a lifetime diagnosis of bipolar I
    (90%) or bipolar II (10%) disorder and their opposite sex partners
    participated in a 6-month weekly diary study. In 71% of cases, the

    individual with bipolar disorder (hereafter referred to as the pa-
    tient) was female. Participants ranged in age from 25 to 64 years,
    with a mean age of 44 years (SD � 10) for patients and 46 years
    (SD � 11) for partners. The sample was predominantly non-
    Hispanic White (92% of patients, 84% of partners), with the
    remainder identifying as Hispanic of any race (5% of patients and
    8% of partners) or other (3% of patients, 8% of partners). Partic-
    ipants had 15 years of education on average (SD � 3 years) and
    50% of patients and 76% of partners were employed, with a
    median household income of $4,500 per month. All couples had
    been living together for at least 1 year, with an average relationship
    length of 12 years (SD � 10), and 84% were married. In 76% of
    couples, at least one partner had a biological child (children’s age
    ranged from 1 to 41 years), with a mean of 2.86 children (SD �
    1.66) among couples who had children.

    Procedure

    The study was conducted in a large southwestern city in the
    United States. All procedures were approved by the local institu-
    tional review board. Couples were recruited through Internet and
    newspaper advertisements and presentations to local mental health
    consumer organizations. To participate, one partner had to meet
    Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth
    edition (DSM–IV; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) crite-
    ria for bipolar I or II disorder, and the other partner could not meet
    criteria for a bipolar spectrum disorder or a primary psychotic
    disorder. The couple had to be married and/or cohabiting for at
    least one year, and partners had to be between the ages of 25 and
    64 years, have completed a tenth-grade education or higher, and be
    able to read and understand English.

    After providing informed consent, participants completed a bat-
    tery of questionnaires and clinical interviews (including those to
    confirm diagnostic eligibility) at a laboratory assessment. At the
    end of the assessment, participants completed the first weekly
    diary, described below, and received instructions for completing
    and returning weekly diaries for the next 6 months. Participants
    received $125 each ($250 per couple) in compensation for com-
    pleting the initial laboratory session and $5 for each completed
    diary. They were asked to complete the weekly diaries indepen-
    dently from their partner and return them in self-addressed,
    stamped envelopes. To encourage timely completion of diaries,
    participants received payment only if the post date of the diary was
    within 3 days of the due date; only data from these diaries were
    included in analyses. Participants completed an average of 20
    weekly diaries (range � 2–26, SD � 8), with 74% completing at
    least 20, 8% completing 10 –20, 9% completing 5–10, and 8%
    completing 4 or fewer.

    Measures

    Diagnosis and symptoms. The Structured Clinical Interview
    for DSM–IV Axis I disorders, research version, patient edition
    (SCID-I/P; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 2002) was used to
    confirm diagnostic eligibility. The SCID was administered by
    clinical psychology doctoral students under the supervision of the
    primary investigator. Patients and their partners completed the
    SCID-I/P independently with different interviewers. The SCID-I/P
    is a reliable and well-validated diagnostic tool (e.g., First, Spitzer,

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    539RELATIONSHIP SATISFACTION IN BPD

    Gibbon, & Williams, 2002). Interrater agreement within this study
    was calculated by rescoring 30% of all interviews (n � 23);
    current mood episode (� � .89), and mood diagnosis (� � .83) had
    acceptable agreement.

    Patient and partner weekly depressive symptoms were assessed
    with the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9; Kroenke, Spitzer,
    & Williams, 2001), a nine-item measure of DSM–IV depressive
    symptoms experienced in the past week. Symptoms were rated on
    a scale ranging from 0, not at all, to 3, nearly every day, with total
    scores ranging from 0 to 27. The PHQ-9 is well validated and
    reliable, with good specificity and sensitivity to change (Kroenke
    et al., 2001). Coefficient alpha for the first diary was .91 for
    patients and .88 for partners. First-week diary scores were corre-
    lated with Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (Hamilton, 1960)
    scores obtained at the laboratory assessment, r � .67 for patients,
    .77 for partners, ps � .001.

    Patient weekly manic symptoms were measured using the Alt-
    man Self Rating Scale for Mania (ASRM; Altman, Hedeker,
    Peterson, & Davis, 1997), a five-item measure of manic symptoms
    in which participants rate symptoms on a scale ranging from 0 to
    4; total scores can range from 0 to 20. The ASRM is correlated
    with clinician-rated measures of mania and has good reliability and
    specificity (Altman et al., 1997). Coefficient alpha for ASRM
    scores in the first diary was .89. First-week diary scores were
    correlated with the Young Mania Rating Scale (Young, Biggs,
    Ziegler, & Meyer, 1978) scores obtained at the laboratory assess-
    ment, r � .77, p � .001.

    Relationship satisfaction. Weekly relationship satisfaction
    was measured by a single item, “All things considered, how happy
    have you felt in your relationship in the last week?” on a nine-point
    scale ranging from 0, very unhappy, to 8, perfectly happy. First-
    week satisfaction scores were positively correlated with self-
    reported relationship satisfaction at the laboratory assessment us-
    ing the Dyadic Adjustment Scale (Spanier, 1976), r � .46 for
    patients and .39 for partners, ps � .05. Previous studies have
    documented the validity of single-item measures of constructs
    such as relationship closeness (Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992), life
    satisfaction (Antonucci, Lansford, & Akiyama, 2001), and well-
    being (Pavot & Diener, 1993).

    Social support. Participants reported on weekly support using
    a single item, “My partner has provided emotional support for
    me,” on a scale ranging from 0, not at all, to 8, very much. Social
    support from the first diary week was correlated with reports of
    overall social support from the partner on the Social Provisions
    Scale (Cutrona & Russell, 1987), obtained at the laboratory as-
    sessment, r � .40 for patients and .57 for partners, ps � .05.
    Although social support and relationship satisfaction are correlated
    and some older measures of relationship satisfaction have included
    items about social support (Fincham & Bradbury, 1987), more
    recent research shows that they are related, but distinct, constructs
    (e.g., Funk & Rogge, 2007).

    Data Analytic Plan

    Multilevel models with distinguishable dyads (patient vs. part-
    ner) across up to 26 weeks of diaries (diary completed at the
    laboratory assessment plus 25 additional diaries) were used to test
    the hypotheses. Data that were missing at random, such as skipped
    individual items in multi-item scales (.01% of the PHQ-9 items

    and .002% of the ASRM items), were imputed using EM imputa-
    tion procedures. Missing single items measuring relationship sat-
    isfaction and social support were not imputed (8.1% of the rela-
    tionship satisfaction items, 0.2% of the social support items)
    because it was impossible to know whether the item was missing
    at random or on purpose.

    Models were estimated in SAS PROC MIXED (SAS Institute,
    Cary, NC) using restricted maximum likelihood. The intraclass
    correlation (as calculated for a dual-intercept empty-means model)
    for relationship satisfaction was .51 for patients and .39 for part-
    ners, indicating that 51% and 39% of the variance in relationship
    satisfaction was due to between-person mean differences in pa-
    tients and partners, respectively, with the remaining variance oc-
    curring at the within-person level. The intraclass correlation for
    social support was .56 for patients and .53 for partners, indicating
    that approximately half of the variance in social support was due
    to between-person mean differences. Thus, examination of within-
    person means for both relationship satisfaction and social support
    was justified.

    We used modified Actor-Partner Interdependence Models
    (Kenny, 1996), including separate fixed and random intercepts for
    patients and partners (Atkins, 2005), as shown in the equation for
    relationship satisfaction below. Independent variables were disag-
    gregated into Level 2 person-mean (PM) and Level 1 within-
    person (WP) components (Singer & Willett, 2003). Person-mean
    variables were grand-mean centered by partner, and WP variables
    were centered at each individual’s mean score. We included both
    actor and partner effects for weekly depressive symptoms but only
    the actor effect of manic symptoms for patients and the partner
    effect for partners because partners, by definition, had very low
    levels of manic symptoms. The autoregressive coefficient for the
    dependent variable (i.e., the individual’s score from the previous
    week) was included in all models to control for the possibility that
    the association between present week satisfaction and support was
    due to the effect of past week values. Patient sex was not included
    in the analyses reported below because it did not moderate effects
    (analyses including sex as a moderator are available from the
    authors upon request).

    Relationship satisfactionti � (patient)��00 � �01(PM social supporti)

    � �02(PM actor PHQ-9i) � �03(PM partner PHQ-9i)

    � �04(PM actor ASRMi) � �10(WP social supportti)

    � �20(WP actor PHQ-9ti) � �30(WP partner PHQ-9ti)

    � �40(WP actor ASRMti) � �50(previous week satisfactionti)

    � ε0i� � (partner)��100 � �101(PM social supporti)

    � �102(PM actor PHQ-9i) � �103(PM partner PHQ-9i)

    ��104(PM partner ASRMi) � �110(WP social supportti)

    � �120(WP actor PHQ-9ti) � �130(WP partner PHQ-9ti)

    � �140(WP partner ASRMti) � �150(previous week satisfactionti)

    � ε10i�

    Results

    Participants reported moderate levels of relationship satisfaction
    (patients: M � 4.71, SD � 2.34; partners: M � 4.76, SD � 2.17)
    and emotional support (patients: M � 5.04, SD � 2.17; partners:
    M � 4.26, SD � 2.26) on average over the course of the study.

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    540 BOYERS AND SIMPSON ROWE

    Patients had moderate symptoms of depression (M � 7.10, SD �
    6.78) and mild symptoms of mania (M � 2.23, SD � 3.55) on
    average, whereas partners had mild symptoms of depression (M �
    1.85, SD � 3.12).

    As expected, a test of the difference of the intercept coefficients
    using an empty-means model revealed that partners reported less
    emotional support than patients, t(37.7) � �2.96, p � .005 (Hy-
    pothesis 1). Next, we tested the hypotheses that support on average
    would be positively associated with relationship satisfaction on
    average (Hypothesis 2) and that support in any given week would
    be associated with concurrent relationship satisfaction in that week
    (Hypothesis 3). We regressed weekly satisfaction onto person-
    mean and within-person support, controlling for past week satis-
    faction and person-mean and within-person psychiatric symptoms.
    As hypothesized, person-mean support was positively associated
    with average relationship satisfaction for patients, b � .51, SE �
    .09, p � .001, and partners, b � .35, SE � .10, p � .002, and
    within-person support was positively associated with within-
    person relationship satisfaction for patients, b � .32, SE � .04,
    p � .001, and partners, b � .33, SE � .04, p � .001 (see Table 1).

    Finally, we tested the converse hypotheses that satisfaction
    would be positively associated with support, on average (Hypoth-
    esis 4), and that satisfaction in any given week would be associated
    with concurrent support (Hypothesis 5), controlling for past week
    support and person-mean and within-person psychiatric symp-
    toms. As expected, person-mean relationship satisfaction was pos-
    itively associated with average support for patients, b � .58, SE �
    .11, p � .001, and partners, b � .34, SE � .14, p � .02, and
    within-person relationship satisfaction was positively associated
    with within-person support for patients, b � .24, SE � .04, p �
    .001, and partners, b � .23, SE � .03, p � .001 (see Table 2).

    Discussion

    As expected, partners received less social support than patients,
    suggesting that partners of individuals with BPD may be at risk for
    inadequate social support in their relationships. Also as hypothe-

    sized, average social support was positively associated with aver-
    age relationship satisfaction, and greater-than-average support
    within any given week was associated with greater-than-average
    relationship satisfaction that week, controlling for patient and
    partner mood symptoms and previous week relationship satisfac-
    tion. The converse hypotheses, with support as the dependent
    variable and person-mean and within-person relationship satisfac-
    tion as the independent variables, were also supported. These
    results are consistent with the literature (e.g., Cutrona & Suhr,
    1994; Sullivan et al., 2010) and expand the existing body of
    knowledge by demonstrating a reciprocal association between
    support and satisfaction. This pattern is consistent with the inti-
    macy process model (Reis & Patrick, 1996), in which support in
    times of vulnerability enhances intimacy, increasing the likelihood
    of future expressions of vulnerability.

    Our results also highlight the important relationship between
    social support and relationship satisfaction among couples in
    which a partner has BPD, over and above the well-documented
    effects of patient and partner mood symptoms on relationship
    functioning (e.g., Lam et al., 2005). Indeed, our results emphasize
    the need to go beyond the focus on patient symptoms and func-
    tioning alone in understanding BPD and to include broader rela-
    tionship outcomes. Specifically, although individuals with BPD
    and their partners are at high risk for relationship distress and
    dissolution (Coryell et al., 1993; Whisman, 2007), the current
    study shows that at least some couples coping with BPD are able
    to sustain high levels of satisfaction. However, the association
    between social support and relationship satisfaction may also
    indicate that low levels of either variable may have reciprocal
    effects, leading to declines in the other. In addition, the lower
    levels of support reported by partners may reflect an imbalance in
    support provision that could contribute to eventual relationship
    distress and caregiver burden (Brock & Lawrence, 2009; Lam et
    al., 2005). Alternatively, it may be that individuals with BPD
    simply need more support than their partners and the results reflect
    the differential need.

    Table 1
    Predicting Relationship Satisfaction by Patient and Partner
    Social Support

    Variable Patient B (SE) Partner B (SE)

    Intercept 3.29��� (.21) 3.42��� (.22)
    Weekly emotional support,

    person-mean .51��� (.09) .35�� (.10)
    Weekly emotional support,

    within-person .32��� (.04) .33��� (.04)
    Control variables

    Previous week satisfaction .29��� (.03) .29��� (.03)
    Own PHQ-9, person-mean �.09�� (.03) .04 (.06)
    Own PHQ-9, within-person �.07��� (.02) �.15��� (.03)
    Own ASRM, person-mean �.03 (.07) —
    Own ASRM, within-person .03 (.02) —
    Partner PHQ-9, person-mean �.003 (.06) �.05 (.03)
    Partner PHQ-9, within-person �.02 (.04) .001 (.02)
    Partner ASRM, person-mean — .004 (.08)
    Partner ASRM, within-person — .05� (.02)

    Note. PHQ-9 � Patient Health Questionnaire; ASRM � Altman Self-
    Rating Scale for Depression.
    � p � .05. �� p � .01. ��� p �.001.

    Table 2
    Predicting Social Support by Patient and Partner
    Relationship Satisfaction

    Variable Patient B (SE) Partner B (SE)

    Intercept 3.88��� (.25) 3.23��� (.23)
    Weekly relationship satisfaction,

    person-mean .58��� (.11) .34� (.14)
    Weekly relationship satisfaction,

    within-person .24��� (.04) .23��� (.03)
    Control variables

    Social support the previous week .23��� (.04) .26��� (.04)
    Own PHQ-9, person-mean .03 (.04) �.01 (.07)
    Own PHQ-9, within-person �.002 (.02) �.12��� (.03)
    Own ASRM, person-mean .02 (.09) —
    Own ASRM, within-person .03 (.09) —
    Partner PHQ-9, person-mean .02 (.07) �.06 (.04)
    Partner PHQ-9, within-person �.07� (.03) �.04�� (.02)
    Partner ASRM, person-mean — .15 (.09)
    Partner ASRM, within-person — .03 (.02)

    Note. PHQ-9 � Patient Health Questionnaire; ASRM � Altman Self-
    Rating Scale for Depression.
    � p � .05. �� p � .01. ��� p � .001.

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    541RELATIONSHIP SATISFACTION IN BPD

    Limitations

    The primary limitation in the current study is the use of single-
    item measures of relationship satisfaction and social support.
    Single-item measures limit the information that can be obtained
    about multifaceted constructs; future studies of social support in
    couples with BPD using more comprehensive measures of both
    variables are important to replicate our findings. In addition, the
    sample was relatively small and is not likely to be representative of
    all couples in which a partner has BPD. Indeed, the low levels of
    mood symptoms, on average, suggest that this may be a relatively
    high-functioning sample, although many patients in the study
    experienced weeks in which depressive and/or manic symptoms
    were quite high. Finally, the majority of participants were White,
    so the results may not generalize to a more diverse sample.

    Implications and Future Directions

    Social support from an intimate partner is highly beneficial (e.g.,
    Cutrona & Suhr, 1994) as long as support is provided with some
    degree of skill and balanced, with neither partner experiencing too
    much burden of support provision or feeling inadequate as a result
    of needing support (Bolger & Amarel, 2007; Brock & Lawrence,
    2009). Our findings extend the literature on social support to
    individuals with BPD and their partners. Unfortunately, couples in
    this population may be less skilled in support provision and ac-
    ceptance than couples without severe mental illness, given the high
    rates of relationship dysfunction in BPD (Coryell et al., 1993; Judd
    & Akiskal, 2003; Whisman, 2007). Future research will need to
    explore the skill with which patients with BPD and their partners
    provide support to each other and factors that may interfere with
    support provision (e.g., severe mood episodes, substance abuse,
    and stress). Experimental manipulation of support provision
    through psychoeducation or instructions may also enhance our
    understanding of the association between support and relationship
    satisfaction within this population. Such research has the potential
    to inform relationship and family-based interventions that may
    benefit individuals with BPD and their loved ones.

    References

    Altman, E. G., Hedeker, D., Peterson, J. L., & Davis, J. M. (1997). The
    Altman self-rating mania scale. Biological Psychiatry, 42, 948 –955.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00548-3

    American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical man-
    ual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Author.

    Antonucci, T. C., Lansford, J. E., & Akiyama, H. (2001). Impact of
    positive and negative aspects of marital relationships and friendships on
    well-being of older adults. Applied Developmental Science, 5, 68 –75.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532480XADS0502_2

    Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self
    scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality
    and Social Psychology, 63, 596 – 612. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
    3514.63.4.596

    Atkins, D. C. (2005). Using multilevel models to analyze couple and
    family treatment data: Basic and advanced issues. Journal of Family
    Psychology, 19, 98 –110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.19.1.98

    Beach, S. R. H., Martin, J. K., Blum, T. C., & Roman, P. M. (1993). Effects
    of marital and co-worker relationships on negative affect: Testing the
    central role of marriage. American Journal of Family Therapy, 21,
    313–323. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01926189308251002

    Bodenmann, G. (1995). A systematic-transactional conceptualization of
    stress and coping in couples. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 54, 34 – 49.

    Bolger, N., & Amarel, D. (2007). Effects of social support visibility on
    adjustment to stress: Experimental evidence. Journal of Personality and
    Social Psychology, 92, 458 – 475. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514
    .92.3.458

    Brock, R. L., & Lawrence, E. (2009). Too much of a good thing: Under-
    provision versus overprovision of partner support. Journal of Family
    Psychology, 23, 181–192. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015402

    Cohen, A. N., Hammen, C., Henry, R. M., & Daley, S. E. (2004). Effects
    of stress and social support on recurrence in bipolar disorder. Journal of
    Affective Disorders, 82, 143–147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2003
    .10.008

    Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering
    hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310 –357. http://dx.doi.org/10
    .1037/0033-2909.98.2.310

    Coryell, W., Scheftner, W., Keller, M., Endicott, J., Maser, J., & Klerman,
    G. L. (1993). The enduring psychosocial consequences of mania and
    depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 720 –727. http://dx.doi
    .org/10.1176/ajp.150.5.720

    Cutrona, C. E., & Russell, D. (1987). The provisions of social relationships
    and adaptation to stress. In W. H. Jones & D. Perlman (Eds.), Vol. 1, pp.
    37– 67). Advances in personal relationships. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

    Cutrona, C. E., & Suhr, J. A. (1992). Controllability of stressful events and
    satisfaction with spouse support behaviors. Communication Research,
    19, 154 –174. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/009365092019002002

    Cutrona, C. E., & Suhr, J. A. (1994). Social support communication in the
    context of marriage: An analysis of couples’ supportive interactions. In
    B. R. Burleson, T. L. Albrecht, & I. G. Sarason (Eds.), Communication
    of social support: Messages, interactions, relationships, and community
    (pp. 113–135). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Dore, G., & Romans, S. E. (2001). Impact of bipolar affective disorder on
    family and partners. Journal of Affective Disorders, 67, 147–158. http://
    dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00450-5

    Dunkel-Schetter, C., & Skokan, L. A. (1990). Determinants of social
    support provision in personal relationships. Journal of Social and Per-
    sonal Relationships, 7, 437– 450. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/02654
    07590074002

    Fagiolini, A., Kupfer, D. J., Masalehdan, A., Scott, J. A., Houck, P. R., &
    Frank, E. (2005). Functional impairment in the remission phase of
    bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 7, 281–285. http://dx.doi.org/10
    .1111/j.1399-5618.2005.00207.x

    Fincham, F. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1987). The assessment of marital
    quality: A reevaluation. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 49, 797–
    809. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/351973

    First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (2002).
    Structured clinical interview for DSM–IV axis I disorders, research
    version. New York, NY: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychi-
    atric Institute.

    Funk, J. L., & Rogge, R. D. (2007). Testing the ruler with item response
    theory: Increasing precision of measurement for relationship satisfaction
    with the Couples Satisfaction Index. Journal of Family Psychology, 21,
    572–583. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.21.4.572

    Haber, M. G., Cohen, J. L., Lucas, T., & Baltes, B. B. (2007). The
    relationship between self-reported received and perceived social sup-
    port: A meta-analytic review. American Journal of Community Psychol-
    ogy, 39, 133–144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10464-007-9100-9

    Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology,
    Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 23, 56 – 62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/
    jnnp.23.1.56

    Howland, M., & Simpson, J. A. (2010). Getting in under the radar. A
    dyadic view of invisible support. Psychological Science, 21, 1878 –1885.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797610388817

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    542 BOYERS AND SIMPSON ROWE

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223%2896%2900548-3

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532480XADS0502_2

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.19.1.98

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01926189308251002

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.458

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.3.458

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015402

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2003.10.008

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2003.10.008

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.150.5.720

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/ajp.150.5.720

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/009365092019002002

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327%2801%2900450-5

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327%2801%2900450-5

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407590074002

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407590074002

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2005.00207.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2005.00207.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/351973

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.21.4.572

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10464-007-9100-9

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797610388817

    Johnson, L., Lundström, O., Åberg-Wistedt, A., & Mathé, A. A. (2003).
    Social support in bipolar disorder: Its relevance to remission and relapse.
    Bipolar Disorders, 5, 129 –137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1034/j.1399-5618
    .2003.00021.x

    Johnson, S. L., Winett, C. A., Meyer, B., Greenhouse, W. J., & Miller, I.
    (1999). Social support and the course of bipolar disorder. Journal of
    Abnormal Psychology, 108, 558 –566. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-
    843X.108.4.558

    Judd, L. L., & Akiskal, H. S. (2003). The prevalence and disability of
    bipolar spectrum disorders in the U.S. population: Re-analysis of the
    ECA database taking into account subthreshold cases. Journal of Affec-
    tive Disorders, 73, 123–131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(02)
    00332-4

    Kenny, D. A. (1996). Models of non-independence in dyadic research.
    Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 13, 279 –294. http://dx
    .doi.org/10.1177/0265407596132007

    Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. (2001). The PHQ-9: Validity
    of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal
    Medicine, 16, 606 – 613. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001
    .016009606.x

    Kulhara, P., Basu, D., Mattoo, S. K., Sharan, P., & Chopra, R. (1999).
    Lithium prophylaxis of recurrent bipolar affective disorder: Long-term
    outcome and its psychosocial correlates. Journal of Affective Disorders,
    54, 87–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(98)00145-1

    Lam, D., Donaldson, C., Brown, Y., & Malliaris, Y. (2005). Burden and
    marital and sexual satisfaction in the partners of bipolar patients. Bipolar
    Disorders, 7, 431– 440. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2005
    .00240.x

    Lee, A. M. R., Simeon, D., Cohen, L. J., Samuel, J., Steele, A., &
    Galynker, I. I. (2011). Predictors of patient and caregiver distress in an
    adult sample with bipolar disorder seeking family treatment. Journal of
    Nervous and Mental Disease, 199, 18 –24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/
    NMD.0b013e3182043b73

    Pavot, W., & Diener, E. (1993). The affective and cognitive context of
    self-reported measures of subjective well-being. Social Indicators Re-
    search, 28, 1–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01086714

    Reinares, M., Vieta, E., Colom, F., Martínez-Arán, A., Torrent, C., Comes,
    M., . . . Sánchez-Moreno, J. (2006). What really matters to bipolar
    patients’ caregivers: Sources of family burden. Journal of Affective
    Disorders, 94, 157–163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2006.04.022

    Reis, H. T., & Patrick, B. C. (1996). Attachment and intimacy: Component
    processes. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychol-
    ogy: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 523–563). New York, NY:
    Guilford Press.

    Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis:
    Modeling change and event occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford Uni-
    versity Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195152968.001
    .0001

    Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for
    assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage
    and the Family, 38, 15–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/350547

    Sullivan, K. T., Pasch, L. A., Johnson, M. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (2010).
    Social support, problem solving, and the longitudinal course of newly-
    wed marriage. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 631–
    644. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017578

    Whisman, M. A. (2007). Marital distress and DSM–IV psychiatric disor-
    ders in a population-based national survey. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
    chology, 116, 638 – 643. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.638

    Whisman, M. A., Uebelacker, L. A., & Weinstock, L. M. (2004). Psycho-
    pathology and marital satisfaction: The importance of evaluating both
    partners. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 830 – 838.
    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.830

    Young, R. C., Biggs, J. T., Ziegler, V. E., & Meyer, D. A. (1978). A rating
    scale for mania: Reliability, validity and sensitivity. British Journal of
    Psychiatry, 133, 429 – 435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.133.5.429

    Received February 9, 2017
    Revision received November 12, 2017

    Accepted November 15, 2017 �

    T
    hi
    s
    do
    cu
    m
    en
    t
    is
    co
    py
    ri
    gh
    te
    d
    by
    th
    e
    A
    m
    er
    ic
    an
    P
    sy
    ch
    ol
    og
    ic
    al
    A
    ss
    oc
    ia
    ti
    on
    or
    on
    e
    of
    it
    s
    al
    li
    ed
    pu
    bl
    is
    he
    rs
    .
    T
    hi
    s
    ar
    ti
    cl
    e
    is
    in
    te
    nd
    ed
    so
    le
    ly
    fo
    r
    th
    e
    pe
    rs
    on
    al
    us
    e
    of
    th
    e
    in
    di
    vi
    du
    al
    us
    er
    an
    d
    is
    no
    t
    to
    be
    di
    ss
    em
    in
    at
    ed
    br
    oa
    dl
    y.

    543RELATIONSHIP SATISFACTION IN BPD

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1034/j.1399-5618.2003.00021.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1034/j.1399-5618.2003.00021.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.108.4.558

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.108.4.558

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327%2802%2900332-4

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327%2802%2900332-4

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407596132007

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0265407596132007

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327%2898%2900145-1

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2005.00240.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1399-5618.2005.00240.x

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182043b73

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0b013e3182043b73

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01086714

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2006.04.022

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195152968.001.0001

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195152968.001.0001

    http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/350547

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017578

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.116.3.638

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.830

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.133.5.429

      Social Support and Relationship Satisfaction in Bipolar Disorder
      Method
      Participants
      Procedure
      Measures
      Diagnosis and symptoms
      Relationship satisfaction
      Social support
      Data Analytic Plan
      Results
      Discussion
      Limitations
      Implications and Future Directions
      References

    Calculate your order
    Pages (275 words)
    Standard price: $0.00
    Client Reviews
    4.9
    Sitejabber
    4.6
    Trustpilot
    4.8
    Our Guarantees
    100% Confidentiality
    Information about customers is confidential and never disclosed to third parties.
    Original Writing
    We complete all papers from scratch. You can get a plagiarism report.
    Timely Delivery
    No missed deadlines – 97% of assignments are completed in time.
    Money Back
    If you're confident that a writer didn't follow your order details, ask for a refund.

    Calculate the price of your order

    You will get a personal manager and a discount.
    We'll send you the first draft for approval by at
    Total price:
    $0.00
    Power up Your Academic Success with the
    Team of Professionals. We’ve Got Your Back.
    Power up Your Study Success with Experts We’ve Got Your Back.

    Order your essay today and save 30% with the discount code ESSAYHELP