Discussion 601
500 words
Describe the major elements of evidence-based policing?
How do the elements relate to research in criminal justice?
In what areas is evidence-based policing already being used?
What are the steps you would take to convince a department not using
evidence-based policing to use it?
Ideas in
American
Policing
By Lawrence W. Sherman
July 1998
Ideas in American Policing presents
commentary and insight from
leading criminologists on issues of
interest to scholars, practitioners,
and policymakers. The papers
published in this series are from the
Police Foundation lecture series of
the same name. Points of view in
this document are those of the
author and do not necessarily
represent the official position of the
Police Foundation.
©1998 Police Foundation and
Lawrence W. Sherman. All rights reserved.
Lawrence W. Sherman is
professor and chair of the
Department of Criminology
and Criminal Justice at the
University of Maryland at
College Park. He was the
Police Foundation’s director
of research from 1979 to
1985.
POLICE
FOUNDATION
Abstract
The new paradigm of “evidence-based medicine” holds
important implications for policing. It suggests that just doing
research is not enough and that proactive efforts are required to
push accumulated research evidence into practice through national
and community guidelines. These guidelines can then focus in-
house evaluations of what works best across agencies, units,
victims, and officers. Statistical adjustments for the risk factors
shaping crime can provide fair comparisons across police units,
including national rankings of police agencies by their crime
prevention effectiveness. The example of domestic violence, for
which accumulated National Institute of Justice research could
lead to evidence-based guidelines, illustrates the way in which
agency-based outcomes research could further reduce violence
against victims. National pressure to adopt this paradigm could
come from agency-ranking studies, but police agency capacity to
adopt it will require new data systems creating “medical charts”
for crime victims, annual audits of crime reporting systems, and
in-house “evidence cops” who document the ongoing patterns
and effects of police practices in light of published and in-house
research. These analyses can then be integrated into the NYPD
Compstat feedback model for management accountability and
continuous quality improvement.
Most of us have thought of the
statistician’s work as that of measuring
and predicting . . . but few of us have
thought it the statistician’s duty to try to
bring about changes in the things that he
[or she] measures.
—W. Edwards Deming
—— 2 ——
Of all the ideas in policing,
one stands out as the most
powerful force for change: police
practices should be based on
scientific evidence about what
works best. Early in this century,
Berkeley Police Chief August
Vollmer’s partnership with his
local university helped generate
this idea (Carte and Carte 1975),
which was clearly derived from
that era’s expansion of the
scientific method into medicine,
management, agriculture, and
many other fields (Cheit 1975).
While science had greater initial
impact in those other professions
during the first half of the
century, policing in recent
decades has been moving rapidly
to catch up. However, any
assessment of this idea in modern
policing must begin with an
accurate benchmark: catching up
to what? More complete evidence
on the linkage between research
and practice suggests a new
paradigm for police improvement
and for public safety in general:
evidence-based crime prevention.
For years, Sherman (1984,
1992) and others have used
medicine as the exemplar of a
profession based upon strong
scientific evidence. Sherman has
praised medicine as a field in
which practitioners have advanced
training in the scientific method
and keep up-to-date with the
most recent research evidence by
reading medical journals. He has
cited the large body of
randomized controlled
experiments in medicine—now
estimated to number almost one
million in print (Sackett and
Rosenberg 1995)—as the highly
rigorous scientific evidence used
to guide medical practices. He
has suggested that policing
should therefore be more like
medicine.
Sherman was right about the
need for many more randomized
experiments in policing, but
wrong about how much medicine
was really based on scientific
research. New evidence shows
that doctors resist changing
practices based on new research
just as much as police do, if not
more so. Closer examination
reveals medicine to be a
battleground between research
and practice, with useful lessons
for policing on new ways to
promote research. Those lessons
come from a new strategy called
“evidence-based medicine,”1
“widely hailed as the long-sought
link between research and
practice” (Zuger 1997) to solve
problems like the following
(Millenson 1997, 4, 122, 131):
• An estimated 85 percent of
medical practices remain
untested by research evidence.
• Most doctors rarely read the
2,500 medical journals
available, and instead base their
practice on local custom.
• Most studies that do guide
practice use weak, non-
randomized research designs.
Medicine, in fact, seems just
as resistant to the use of evidence
to guide practice as are fields with
lower educational requirements,
such as policing. The National
Institutes of Health (NIH)
Consensus Guidelines are a case
in point. NIH convenes advisory
boards to issue to physicians
recommendations that are based
on intensive reviews of research
evidence on specific medical
practices. These recommendations
usually receive extensive publicity,
and are reinforced by mailings of
the guideline summaries to some
one hundred thousand doctors.
But according to a RAND
evaluation, doctors rarely change
their practices in response to
publication of these guidelines
(Kosecoff et al. 1987, as cited in
Millenson 1997). Thus three
years after research found that
heart attack patients treated with
calcium antagonists were more
likely to die, doctors still
prescribed this dangerous drug to
one-third of heart attack patients.
Eight years after antibiotics were
shown to cure ulcers, 90 percent
of ulcer patients remained
untreated by antibiotics
(Millenson 1997, 123–25).
Evidence Cops
The struggle to change
medical practice based on
research evidence has a long
history, with valuable implications
for policing. In the 1840s, Ignaz
Semmelweiss found evidence that
maternal death in childbirth
could be reduced if doctors
1 The term “evidence” in this mono-
graph refers to scientific, not criminal,
evidence.
—— 3 ——
washed their hands before
delivering babies. He then tried to
apply this research to medical
practice in Vienna, which led to
his being driven out of town by
his boss, the chief obstetrician.
Hundreds of thousands of women
died because the profession
refused to comply with his
evidence-based guidelines for
some forty years. The story shows
the important distinction between
merely doing research and
attempting to apply research to
redirect professional practices.
One way to describe people
who try to apply research is the
role of “evidence cop.”
More like
a traffic cop than Victor Hugo’s
detective Javert, the evidence
cop’s job is to redirect practice
through compliance rather than
punishment. While this job may
be as challenging as herding cats,
it still consists of pointing
professionals to practice “this way,
not that way.” As in all policing,
the success rate for this job varies
widely. Fortunately, the initial
failures of people like Semmelweiss
paved the way for greater success
in the 1990s.
Consider Scott Wein-
garten, M.D., of Cedars-Sinai
Hospital in Los Angeles. As
director of the hospital’s Center
for Applied Health Services
Research, Weingarten is an
evidence-cop-in-residence. His
job is to monitor what the 2,25
0
doctors are doing to patients at
the hospital and to detect
practices that run counter to
recommendations based on
research evidence. He does this
through prodding rather than
punishment, convening groups of
doctors who treat specific
maladies to discuss the research
evidence. These groups then
produce their own consensus
guidelines for practices that
become hospital policy. Thirty-
five such sets of guidelines were
produced in Weingarten’s first
four years on the job (Millenson
1997, 120).
What NIH, Weingarten, and
the 1995 founders of the new
journal called Evidence-Based
Medicine are all trying to do is to
push research into practice. Just
as policing has become more
proactive at dealing with crime,
researchers are becoming more
proactive about dealing with
practice. This trend has developed
in many fields, not just medicine.
Increased pressure for
“reinventing government” to
focus on measurable results is
reflected in the 1994 U.S.
Government Performance Results
Act (GPRA), which requires all
federal agencies to file annual
reports on quantitative indicators
of their achievements. Education
is under growing pressure to raise
test scores as proof that children
are learning, which has led to
increased discussion of research
evidence on what works in
education (Raspberry 1998). And
the U.S. Congress has required
that the effectiveness of federally
funded crime prevention
programs be evaluated using
“rigorous and scientifically
recognized standards and
methodologies” (House 1995,
sec. 116). All this sets the stage
for a new paradigm for making
research more useful to policing
than it has ever been before.
Key Questions
In suggesting a new paradigm
called evidence-based policing,
there are four key questions to
answer: What is it? What is new
about it? How does it apply to a
specific example of police
practice?
How can it be
institutionalized?
What is it?
Evidence-based policing is the
use of the best available research
on the outcomes of police work
to implement guidelines and
evaluate agencies, units, and
officers. Put more simply,
One way to describe people
who try to apply research is the
role of “evidence cop.”
—— 4 ——
evidence-based policing uses
research to guide practice and
evaluate practitioners. It uses the
best evidence to shape the best
practice. It is a systematic effort
to parse out and codify
unsystematic “experience” as the
basis for police work, refining it
by ongoing systematic testing of
hypotheses.
Evaluation of ongoing
operations has been the crucial
missing link in many recent
attempts to improve policing. If it
is true that most police work has
yet to go “beyond 911”
(Sparrow, Moore, and Kennedy
1990), the underlying reason may
be a lack of evaluation systems
that clearly link research-based
guidelines to outcomes. It is only
with that addition that policing
can become a “reflexive” or
“smart” institution, continuously
improving with ongoing
feedback.
The basic premise of
evidence-based practice is that we
are all entitled to our own
opinions, but not to our own
facts. Yet left alone to practice
individually, practitioners do
come up with their own “facts,”
which often turn out to be
wrong. A recent survey of 82
Washington State doctors found
137 different strategies for
treating urinary tract infections
(Berg 1991). No doubt the same
result could be found for
handling domestic disturbances.
A study evaluating the accuracy
of strep throat diagnoses based
on unstructured examination by
experienced pediatricians found it
far inferior to a systematic,
evidence-based checklist used by
nurses. The mythic power of
subjective and unstructured
wisdom holds back every field
and keeps it from systematically
discovering and implementing
what works best in repeated tasks.
A prime example of the
power of systematic, ongoing
evaluations comes again from
medicine. In 1990, the New York
State Health Department began
to publish death rates for
coronary bypass surgery grouped
by hospital and individual
surgeon. This action was
prompted by research showing
that while the statewide average
death rate was 3.7 percent, some
doctors ran as high as 82 percent.
Moreover, after adjusting for the
risk of death by the pre-operation
condition of the patient caseload,
patients were 4.4 times more
likely to die in surgery at the least
successful hospitals than at the
best hospitals. Despite enormous
opposition from hospitals and
surgeons, these data were made
public, revealing a strong practice
effect: the more operations
doctors and hospitals did each
year, the lower the risk-adjusted
death rate. Using this clear
correlation to push low-frequency
surgeons and hospitals out of this
business altogether, hospitals
were able to lower the death rate
in these operations by 40 percent
in just three years (Millenson
1997, 195).
Evidence-based policing is
about two very different kinds of
research: basic research on what
works best when implemented
properly under controlled
conditions, and ongoing
outcomes research about the
results each unit is actually
achieving by applying (or
ignoring) basic research in
practice. This combination creates
a feedback loop (fig. 1) that
begins with either published or
in-house studies suggesting how
policing might obtain the best
effects. The review of this
evidence can lead to guidelines
taking law, ethics, and community
Figure 1. Evidence-Based Policing.
Literature
Best
Evidence
In-House
Guidelines
Outputs
Outcomes
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
➤
—— 5 ——
culture into account. These
guidelines would specify
measurable “outputs,” or
practices that police are asked to
follow. Their varying degrees of
success at delivering those
outputs can then be assessed by
tracking risk-adjusted
“outcomes,” or results over a
reasonably long follow-up period.
These outcomes may be defined
in several different ways: offenses
per 1,000 residents, repeat
victimizations per 100 victims,
repeat offending per
100
offenders, and so on. The
observation that some units are
getting better results than others
can be used to further identify
factors associated with success,
which can then be fed back as
new in-house research to refine
the guidelines and raise the
overall success level of the agency.
Such research could also be
published in national journals or
at least kept in an agency
database as institutional memory
about success and failure rates for
different methods.
What is new about it?
Skeptics may say that there is
nothing new in evidence-based
policing, and that other
paradigms already embrace these
principles. On closer examination,
however, we will see that no
other paradigm contains the
principles for its own
implementation. No other
paradigm contains a principle for
both changing practices and
measuring the success of those
changes with risk-adjusted
outcomes research (like bypass
surgery death rates). No other
paradigm—not even NYPD’s
Computerized Crime Comparison
Statistics (Compstat) strategy
(Bratton with Knobler 1998)—
uses scientific evidence to hold
professionals accountable for
results in peer-reviewed and even
public discussions of outcomes
evidence.
Evidence-based policing is
clearly different from, but very
helpful to, all three present
paradigms of policing. Incident-
specific policing, or 911
responses, currently lack any
outcomes measure except time
out of service. Police officers who
take too much time to handle a
call are sometimes accused of
shirking and are urged by
supervisors to work faster.2 But
no one tracks the rate of repeat
calls by officer or unit to see how
effective the first response was in
preventing future problems.
Evidence-based policing could
use such outcomes to justify
longer time spent on each call on
the basis of an officer’s average
results, rather than issuing a
crude demand that he or she stay
within an average time limit. It
could also place much more
emphasis on learning how to deal
with each call most effectively
and preventively, a question that
currently gets little attention.
Community policing,
however defined, is not clearly
linked to evidence about
effectiveness in preventing crime.
It is much more about how to do
police work—a set of outputs—
than it is about desired results, or
outcomes. Working with the
community and listening to and
respecting community members
are all important elements of the
paradigm. But that paradigm
alone has been easy for many
officers to ignore. Adding the
accountability systems from the
paradigm of evidence-based
policing could actually make
police far more active in working
with the community.
Problem-oriented policing is
clearly the major source for
2 This sounds oddly like the pressure
for drive-in, drive-out childbirth health
insurance now barred by federal law.
Evidence-based policing is
clearly different from, but
very helpful to, all three
present paradigms of policing.
—— 6 ——
evidence-based policing. Herman
Goldstein’s writings (1979,
1990), as well as John Eck and
William Spelman’s SARA model
(1987), clearly emphasize
assessment of problem-solving
responses as a key part of the
process. Yet there is no clear
statement about the use of
scientific evidence either in
selecting strategies for responding
to problems or in monitoring the
implementation and results of
those strategies (Sherman 1991).
Reports on problem-oriented
policing have so far produced
little evidence either from
controlled tests or outcomes
research. Because the paradigm
stresses the unique characteristics
of each crime pattern, problem-
oriented policing has not been
used to respond to highly
repetitive situations like domestic
assaults or disputes. Few
comparisons of different methods
for attacking the same problem
have been developed. Few officers
are even held accountable for not
implementing a problem-solving
plan they have agreed to
undertake. Problem-oriented
policing has clearly revolutionized
the way many police think about
their objectives, moving them
away from a narrow focus on
each incident to a broader focus
on patterns and systems. But in
the absence of pressure from an
evidence-based approach to
evaluating success and
management accountability,
problem-oriented policing has
been kept at the margins of
police work.
NYPD’s Compstat strategy
(Bratton with Knobler 1998) has
pushed the results accountability
principle farther than ever before,
but it has not used the scientific
method to assess cause and effect.
Successful managers are
rewarded, but successful methods
are not pinpointed and codified.
What evidence-based policing
adds to these paradigms is a new
principle for decision making:
scientific evidence. Most police
practice, like medical practice, is
still shaped by local custom,
opinions, theories, and subjective
impressions. Evidence-based
policing challenges those
principles of decision making and
creates systematic feedback to
provide continuous quality
improvement in the achievement
of police objectives (see Hoover
1996). Hence the inspiration for
this paradigm is not only
medicine and its randomized
trials, but also the principles of
quality control in manufacturing
developed by Walter Shewhart
(1939) and W. Edwards Deming
(1986). These principles were
initially rejected by U.S. business
leaders, but were finally embraced
in the 1980s after Japanese
industries used them to far
surpass U.S. manufacturers in the
quality of their products.
What makes both policing
and medicine different from
manufacturing, of course, is the
far greater variability in the raw
material to be processed—human
beings. That is what gives the
gold standard of evaluation
research, the randomized
controlled trial, both its strength
and its limitations. The strength
of the research design, pioneered
in policing by the Police
Foundation, is its ability to
reduce uncertainty about the
average effects of a policy on vast
numbers of people. The
limitation of the research design
is that it cannot escape variability
in treatments, responses, and
implementation.
The variability of treatments
in policing is much like that in
surgery, which stands in sharp
contrast to pharmaceuticals.
While the chemical content of
medical drugs is almost always
identical, the procedural content
of surgery varies widely. Similarly,
the style and tone each officer
brings to a citizen encounter
varies enormously and can make a
big difference in the outcome of
a specific case. Dosage, timing,
and follow-up of both drugs and
police work can vary widely in
practice.
Even holding treatment
constant, there is evidence that
both patients and offenders
respond to treatments with wide
variations. Some of these
responses, allergic reactions, can
kill some people with treatments
that cure most others. Offenders
are known to vary in their
responses to police actions by
individual, neighborhood, and
city. And implementation of new
practices based on controlled
experiments in both medicine and
policing varies according to how
well research is communicated,
how much information is created
—— 7 ——
about whether practices actually
change, and how much
reinforcement there is for the
change, both positive and
negative.
Evidence-based policing
assumes that experiments alone
are not enough. Putting research
into practice requires just as
much attention to
implementation as it does to
controlled evaluations. Ongoing
systems for researching
implementation can close the
feedback loop to create the
principle of industrial quality
improvement.
How does it apply to a specific
example of police practice?
The policing of domestic
violence offers a clear illustration
of what is new about the
evidence-based paradigm.
Domestic violence has been the
subject of more police practices
research than any other crime
problem. The research has
arguably had little effect on
police practice, at least by the
new standards of evidence-based
medicine. Yet the available
evidence offers a fair and
scientifically valid approach for
holding police agencies, units,
and officers accountable for the
results of police work, as
measured by repeated domestic
violence against the same victims.
The National Institute of
Justice (NIJ) and the Police
Foundation have provided
policing with extensive
information on what works to
prevent repeated violence. The
research has also shown that, like
surgery, police practices vary
greatly in their implementation.
These variations in practice cause
varying results for repeat
offending against victims. Even
holding practice constant,
responses to arrest vary by
offender, neighborhood, and city.
Finally, research shows very poor
compliance with mandatory arrest
guidelines after they are adopted
(Ferraro 1989).
There are many varieties of
arrest for misdemeanor domestic
violence. The offender may or
may not be handcuffed, arrested
in front of family and neighbors,
given a chance to explain his
version of events to the police, or
treated with courtesy and
politeness. Do these variations on
the theme of arrest make a
difference? They should,
according to the “defiance”
theory of criminal sanction effects
(Sherman 1993). And they did in
Milwaukee, according to
Raymond Paternoster and his
colleagues (1997). The
Milwaukee evidence reveals that
controlling for other risk factors
among some 800 arrested
offenders, those who felt they
were not treated in a procedurally
fair and polite manner were
60 percent more likely to commit
a reported act of domestic
violence in the future (fig. 2).
This finding suggests three ways
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
40%
50%
25%
Fair Unfair
Figure 2. Repeat Domestic Violence and Police Fairness.
Source: Paternoster, et al.
—— 8 ——
to push research into practice:
1) change the guidelines for
making domestic violence arrests
to include those elements that
would enable offenders to
perceive more “procedural
justice”; 2) hold police
accountable for using these
guidelines by comparing rates of
repeat victimization associated
with different police units; and
3) compute these rates using
statistical adjustments for the pre-
existing level of recidivism risks.
The NIJ research provides
other evidence for ways that
police can reduce repeat
offending in misdemeanor
domestic violence. Rather than a
one-size-fits-all policy, the
evidence suggests specific guide-
lines to be used under different
conditions. Offenders who are
absent when police arrive—as
they are in some 40 percent of
cases—respond more effectively
to arrest warrants than offenders
who are arrested on the scene
(Dunford 1990). Offenders who
are employed are deterred by
arrest, while offenders who are
unemployed generally increase
their offending more if they are
arrested than if they are handled
in some other fashion (Pate and
Hamilton 1992; Berk et al. 1992;
Sherman and Smith 1992).
Offenders who live in urban areas
of concentrated poverty commit
more repeat offenses if they are
arrested than if not, while
offenders who live in more
affluent areas commit fewer
repeat offenses if they are arrested
(Marciniak 1994). All of these
findings could be changed by
further research, but for the
moment they are the best
evidence available.
This research evidence could
support guidelines for policing
domestic violence that differed by
neighborhood and absence or
presence of the offender. It could
also support guidelines about
listening to suspects’ side of the
story before making arrest
decisions and generally treating
suspects with courtesy. Other
evidence, such as the extremely
high-risk period for repeat
victimization in the first days and
weeks after the last police
encounter (Strang and Sherman
1996), could be used to fashion
new problem-oriented strategies.
Most important, the existing
research can be used to create a
fair system for evaluating police
performance on the basis of risk-
adjusted outcomes. That evidence
(fig. 3) shows that the likelihood
of a repeat offense is strongly
linked to the number of previous
offenses each offender has.
Once the risk of repeat
offending can be predicted with
reasonable accuracy, it becomes
possible to use those predictions
as a benchmark for police
performance. Just as in the bypass
surgery death rates in New York,
the outcomes of policing can be
Figure 3. Risk of Repeat Domestic Assault by Priors.
Milwaukee Domestic Violence Experiment
0
20
40
60
80
0 1 2 3 7
Percent Repeats
42%
48%
75%
60%
—— 9 ——
controlled for the risk level
inherent in the caseload they face.
Using a citywide database of all
domestic assaults, now running
over ten thousand cases per year
in cities like Milwaukee, a model
can be constructed to assess the
risk of repeat offending in each
case. The overall mix of cases in
each police precinct or for each
officer can generate an average
risk level for that caseload. Each
police patrol district can then be
evaluated according to the actual
versus predicted rate of repeat
offending each year (fig. 4). All
patrol districts in the city can
then be compared on the basis of
their relative percentage
difference between expected and
actual rates of repeat domestic
assault (fig. 5).
By constructing information
systems for this kind of outcome
research, police departments can
focus on an objective that has
only previously been measured in
major experiments. Making the
goal of policing each domestic
assault the outcome of a reduced
repeat offending rate rather than
the output of whether an arrest is
made would have several effects.
One is that crime prevention
would get greater attention than
retribution for its own sake.
While not everyone would
welcome that, it is consistent with
at least some police leaders’ view
of the purpose of the police as a
crime prevention agency (Bratton
with Knobler 1998). Another
effect would be to seek out and
Figure 4. Observed vs. Expected Risk of Repeat
Domestic Violence.
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
Observed Expected
Percent Repeat
25%
50%
-100
-50
0
50
100
150
200
PCT 1 PCT 2 PCT 3 PCT 4 PCT 5
Percent Repeat
Figure 5. Observed vs. Expected Ranking by Precinct.
–50%
–25%
50%
150%
—— 10 ——
even initiate more research on
what works best to prevent
domestic violence. In the world
as we now know it, no one in
policing—from the police chief to
the rookie officer—has any direct
incentive to reduce repeat
offending against known victims.
No one in policing is held
accountable for accomplishing, or
even measuring, that objective.
As a result, no one knows
whether repeat victimization rates
get better or worse from year to
year. Using outcomes evidence to
evaluate performance would make
police practices far more victim-
centered, the top priority being
that of preventing any further
assaults.
How can it be
institutionalized?
The strongest claim about
evidence-based policing is that it
contains the principles of its own
implementation. The principles of
using evidence both to change
and evaluate practice can be
applied to a broad institutional
analysis of implementation. Thus
while the changes described
above would have to occur one
police agency at a time, there are
certain national forces that can
help start the ball rolling. This
can be seen, for example, in
national rankings of big-city
police agencies, as well as national
mandates for improving police
data systems to provide better
evidence. Yet even such external
pressures will not succeed
without internal evidence cops to
import, apply, and create research
evidence.
No institution is likely to
increase voluntarily its
accountability except under
strong external pressure. It is
unlikely that evidence-based
policing could be adopted by a
police executive simply because it
appears to be a good idea. The
history of evidence-based
medicine and education strongly
suggests that professionals will
only make such changes under
external coercion. Nothing seems
to foster such pressure as much as
performance rankings across
agencies (Millenson 1997;
Steinberg 1998). Just as various
public performance measures
allow stockbrokers to rank
publicly-held corporations and
provide those companies with
strong incentives for better
results, public information about
police performance would create
the strongest pressure for
improvement.3
One example of how the
major city police departments
could be ranked on performance
can be found in their homicide
rates, which already receive
extensive publicity. What these
statistics lack, however, is any
scientific analysis of expected risk.
Police performance has nothing
to do, at least in the short run,
with the social, economic,
demographic, and drug market
forces that help shape a city’s
homicide rate. While police
performance may also affect those
homicide rates, the other factors
must be taken into account.
Using risk-adjusted homicide
rates provides one indication of
how well a police department
may be doing things like
confiscating illegal weapons,
patrolling hot spots, regulating
violent taverns and drug markets,
and monitoring youth gangs.
While the basic research literature
would increasingly provide a
source of guidance for taking
initiatives against homicide, a
3 The 1919 results of the first
national rankings of hospitals were
deemed so threatening that the American
College of Surgeons decided to burn the
report immediately in the furnace of
New York’s Waldorf-Astoria Hotel
(Millenson 1997, 146).
The strongest claim about
evidence-based policing is that
it contains the principles of
its own implementation.
—— 11 ——
risk-adjusted outcomes analysis
(fig. 6) would indicate how well
that research had been put into
practice.4
If a credible national research
organization would produce such
“league rankings” among big-city
police departments each year (like
the U.S. News & World Report
rankings of colleges and
universities), the predictable
result in the short term would be
attacks on the methodology used.
That is, in fact, what continues to
go on in New York with the
death rates in surgery. But the
New York rankings have spread
to other states, and consumers
have found them quite valuable.
Doctors—and police—may also
find rankings very valuable in the
long run. Both professions should
enjoy greater public respect as
they get better at producing the
results their consumers want.
The more seriously
performance indicators influence
the fate of organizations, the
more likely they are to be
subverted. Recent examples
include the U.S. Postal Service in
West Virginia, where an elaborate
scheme to defeat the on-time
mail delivery audit was recently
alleged (McAllister 1998). Other
examples include teachers helping
students to cheat on their answers
to national achievement tests and,
of course, police departments
under-reporting crime. The New
York City police have removed
three commanders in the past five
years for improperly counting
crime to make their performance
look better (Kocieniewski 1998),
and several chiefs of police
elsewhere have been convicted on
criminal charges for similar
conduct.
Quite apart from pressures to
corrupt data, criminologists have
long known that police crime
reporting is not reliable, with the
possible exception of homicide.
No two agencies classify crime
the same way. The same event
may be called an aggravated
assault in one agency and a
“miscellaneous incident” in
another. The recent FBI decision
to drop Philadelphia from the
national crime reporting program
was not an isolated action. In
1988, the FBI quietly dropped
the entire states of Florida and
Kentucky. Since the FBI lacks
resources to do on-site audits in
each police agency every year,
these examples are just the tip of
a very big iceberg. There are
already rising suspicions of police
manipulation of crime data as
4 While many of the basic risk factors
would be computed from Census data
that could be out of date by the middle
of each decade, other risk data can be
derived from annually updated sources,
such as the NIJ ADAM data on drug
abuse among arrestees. Unemployment,
school dropout, teen childbirth, and
infant mortality data are also available
annually for each city and could help
predict the expected rate of homicide.
Hypothetical Data
Figure 6. Homicide by City, Actual vs. Predicted.
-60
-40
-20
0
20
40
60
80
NYC Balt. Chi LA Dallas
Percent Difference
–50%
–25%
25%
60%
NYC Baltimore Chicago LA Dallas
—— 12 ——
crime rates fall in many cities.
More serious pressure from
national rankings would threaten
data integrity even more.
One viable solution to this
problem is a federal requirement
for police departments to retain
CPA firms to produce annual
audits of their reported crime
data. This requirement could be
imposed as a condition for
receiving federal funds, just as
many other federal mandates have
already done. Anticipating court
challenges about unfunded
mandates (such as the Brady Bill),
Congress could also provide
funds to pay for the audits.
Crime counting standards could
be set nationally by the
accounting profession in
collaboration with the FBI.
Alternatively, each state legislature
could require (or even fund)
these audits as a means of
assuring fairness in performance
rankings of police departments
within the state. State agencies
such as the criminal justice
statistical centers could also
produce such rankings as a
service to taxpayers. States already
have the option of spending
federal funds on such a purpose
under the broad category of
evaluation funds.
In the process of revitalizing
crime data integrity, there would
be great value in reorganizing
police data systems. Most
important would be the creation
of a “medical chart” for each
crime victim. Like computerized
patient records, this chart would
show the diagnosis (offense
description) for each incident a
victim presents to a police agency,
perhaps anywhere in the state.
The chart would also show what
police did in response, everything
from taking an offense report to
arresting an offender whose
release date from prison is also
kept, updated, in the
computerized victim chart. This
information tool could help
develop many proactive police
methods for preventing repeat
victimization. Allowing officers to
use these data to keep their own
private “batting averages” for
repeat victimization (even
without adjusting for risk) may
encourage them to become
involved and committed to doing
a better job at preventing crime.
Better records are also needed
about what police do about crime
according to certain patterns of
offenses. “Medical charts” for
violent taverns, frequently robbed
convenience stores, and other hot
spots where most crime occurs
would be very useful for ongoing
problem-oriented policing
attempts to reduce repeat
offending at those places. Similar
records could be kept about a
pattern of crimes spread out
across a wider area, such as
automatic teller machine
robberies. If officer teams or units
identify these places or patterns as
crime targets and designate a
control group, these medical
charts can become the basis for
estimating how much crime each
police unit has prevented.
Computers can also help
police officers to implement
practice guidelines. Medical
computer systems now offer
recommended practice guidelines
in response to a checklist of data,
as well as warning when drug
prescriptions fall outside
programmed parameters of
disease type and dosage. The use
of hand-held computers to advise
officers in the field and to provide
instant quality control checks may
not happen soon, but the growth
of police research may make it
inevitable in the long run.
Doctors are not expected to keep
In the process of revitalizing
crime data integrity, there
would be great value in
reorganizing police data
systems.
—— 13 ——
large amounts of research data in
their heads, nor even medical
guidelines for each diagnosis.
Computers will not replace good
judgment, but they can clearly
enhance it.
Federal rules could also
require police departments to
appoint a certified police
criminologist (either internally or
in partnership with a university or
research organization), who
would become the agency’s
evidence cop. Like Scott
Weingarten of Cedars-Sinai, the
departmental criminologist would
be responsible for putting
research into practice, then
evaluating the results. Whether
the criminologist is actually an
employee or a university professor
working in partnership with the
police may not matter as much as
the role itself. The criminologist
could help develop more effective
guidelines for preventing repeat
offending, and could develop
expected versus actual repeat
offending data by offense type for
each police district or detective
unit. A criminologist could add
the scientific method to the
NYPD Compstat process
(Bratton with Knobler 1998),
providing statistics at each
meeting on each patrol district’s
crime trends and patterns (or
even its complaints against police
officers) in relation to the
district’s risk level. Building the
capacity to import, apply, and
create evidence within each police
agency may be an essential
ingredient in the success of this
paradigm.
We may also find that the
traditional distance between
researchers and police officials
shrinks when researchers provide
more immediate managerial
information. Criminologists have
long refused to provide police
managers with data on particular
officers, deeming it contrary to
the ethics of basic research
(Hartnett 1998). By finally
providing the data in a
scientifically reasonable format,
criminologists may become far
more effective at pushing research
into practice.
Criminologists can also act on
the finding that doctors tend to
change practices based on
personal interaction and repeated
computerized feedback, and not
from conferences, classes, or
written research reports
(Millenson 1997, 127–30).
Similar findings have been
published about the effectiveness
of agricultural extension services,
in which university scientists visit
farms and show farmers new
techniques for improving their
crop yields. They echo a Chinese
proverb: Tell me and I will
forget; show me and I will
remember; involve me and I will
understand.
The one test of this principle
in policing to date is Alex Weiss’s
(1997) research on how police
departments adopt innovations.
Based on a national survey of
police chiefs and their top aides,
Weiss discovered that telephone
calls from agency to agency
played a vital role in spreading
new ideas. While written reports
may have supplemented the
phone calls, word-of-mouth
seems to be the major way in
which police innovations are
communicated and adopted.
Weiss’s study suggests the
great importance of gathering
more evidence on evidence. The
empirical question for research is,
what practices work best to
change practices? This inherently
reflexive posture may lead us to
empirical comparisons of the
effectiveness of, for example, NIJ
conferences, mass mailings of
research-in-brief reports, or new
one-on-one approaches. One
example of the latter would be
proactive telephone calls to police
agencies around the U.S. made
by present or former police
officers; callers could be trained
by research organizations to
describe new research findings. If
national consensus guidelines for
practice were developed by panels
of police executives and
The empirical
question for
research is,
what practices
work best to
change
practices?
—— 14 ——
researchers, the callers could
communicate those as well. Other
approaches worth testing might
include field demonstrations in
police technique. This training
would not be based on
experience, as is the current Field
Training Officer system, but
rather it would be based on
evidence that the method being
demonstrated has been proven
effective in reducing repeat
offending.
Conclusion
The test of this paradigm’s
results is not whether it is
adopted this year or in twenty
years. As Lord Keynes has
suggested, the influence of ideas
may be far more glacial than
volcanic. The pressure for better
measures of results is in the spirit
of the age, and police cannot
long escape it. All this paper does
is add one inch to the glacier, so
that we can say of policing what
Dr. William Mayo of the Mayo
Clinic said of his profession
almost a century ago: “The glory
of medicine is that it is constantly
moving forward, that there is
always something more to learn.”
References
Berg, A.O. 1991. Variations
among family physicians’
management strategies for
lower urinary tract infections
in women: A report from the
Washington Physicians’
Collaborative Research
Network. Journal of the
American Board of Family
Practice (September–
October): 327–30.
Berk, Richard A.; Alec Campbell;
Ruth Klap; and Bruce
Western. 1992. The deterrent
effect of arrest in incidents of
domestic violence: A Bayesian
analysis of four field
experiments. American
Sociological Review 57: 698–
708.
Bratton, William, with Peter
Knobler. 1998. Turnaround:
How America’s top cop
reversed the crime epidemic.
New York: Random House.
Carte, Gene, and Elaine Carte.
1975. Police reform in the
United States: The era of
August Vollmer. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Cheit, Earl. 1975. The useful arts
and the liberal tradition. New
York: McGraw-Hill.
Deming, W. Edwards. 1986. Out
of the crisis. Cambridge:
Massachusetts Institute of
Technology, Center for
Advanced Engineering Study.
Dunford, Franklyn. 1990.
System-initiated warrants for
suspects of misdemeanor
domestic assault: A pilot
study. Justice Quarterly 7:
631–53.
Eck, John, and William Spelman.
1987. Problem-solving:
Problem-oriented policing in
Newport News. Washington,
D.C.: Police Executive
Research Forum.
Ferraro, Kathleen J. 1989.
Policing woman battering.
Social Problems 36: 61–74.
Goldstein, Herman. 1979.
Improving policing: A
problem-oriented approach.
Crime and Delinquency 25:
236–58.
———. 1990. Problem-oriented
policing. New York: McGraw-
Hill.
Hartnett, Susan. 1998. Address
to the Third National
Institute of Justice
Conference on Police-
Research Partnerships,
February.
Hodge, Melville H. 1990. Direct
use by physicians of the TDS
Medical Information System.
In A History of Medical
Informatics, edited by
Bruce I. Blum and Karen
Duncan. New York: ACM.
Kosecoff, Jacqueline, et al. 1987.
Effect of the National
Institutes of Health
Consensus Development
Program on physician
practice. Journal of the
American Medical Association
258 (November 20): 2708–
13.
McAllister, Bill. 1998. A “special”
delivery in West Virginia:
Postal employees cheat to
beat rating system.
Washington Post, 10 January,
A1.
Marciniak, Elizabeth. 1994.
Community policing of
domestic violence:
Neighborhood differences in
the effect of arrest. Ph.D.
diss., University of Maryland.
Millenson, Michael L. 1997.
Demanding medical excellence:
Doctors and accountability in
—— 15 ——
the information age. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Office of Technology Assessment
of the Congress of the United
States. 1983. The impact of
randomized clinical trials on
health policy and medical
practice. Background paper
OTA-BP-H-22. Washington,
D.C.: Government Printing
Office.
Pate, Antony M., and Edwin E.
Hamilton. 1992. Formal and
informal deterrents to
domestic violence: The Dade
County Spouse Assault
Experiment. American
Sociological Review 57: 691–
98.
Paternoster, Ray; Bobby Brame;
Ronet Bachman; and
Lawrence W. Sherman. 1997.
Do fair procedures matter?
Procedural justice in the
Milwaukee Domestic Violence
Experiment. Law and Society
Review.
Raspberry, William. 1998. Tried,
true and ignored. Washington
Post, 2 February, A19.
Sackett, David L., and William
M.C. Rosenberg. 1995. On
the need for evidence-based
medicine. Health Economics 4:
249–54.
Sherman, Lawrence W. 1984.
Experiments in police
discretion: Scientific boon or
dangerous knowledge? Law
and Contemporary Problems
47, no. 4: 61–81.
———. 1992. Policing domestic
violence: Experiments and
dilemmas. New York: Free
Press.
———. 1993. Defiance,
deterrence and irrelevance: A
theory of the criminal
sanction. Journal of Research
in Crime and Delinquency 30:
445–73.
——— and Douglas A. Smith.
1992. Crime, punishment and
stake in conformity: Legal
and informal control of
domestic violence. American
Sociological Review 57.
Shewhart, Walter A. 1939.
Statistical methods from the
viewpoint of quality control.
Edited by W.E. Deming.
Lancaster, Pennsylvania:
Graduate School of the U.S.
Department of Agriculture.
Sparrow, Malcolm; Mark Moore;
and David Kennedy. 1990.
Beyond 911: A new era for
policing. New York: Basic
Books.
Steinberg, Jacques. 1998. Public
shaming: Rating system for
schools: Some states are
finding that humiliation leads
to improvement. New York
Times, 7 January, A19.
Strang, Heather, and Lawrence
W. Sherman. 1996. Predicting
domestic homicide. Paper
presented to the American
Association for the
Advancement of Science,
January, in Baltimore,
Maryland.
U.S. Congress. House. 104th
Congress, 1st sess., H. Rept.
104-387, sec. 116.
Weiss, Alexander. 1992. Diffusion
of innovations in police
departments. Ph.D. diss.,
Northwestern University.
Zuger, Abigail. 1997. New way
of doctoring: By the book.
New York Times, 16
December, C1.
—— 16 ——
1201 Connecticut A venue, NW, Washington, DC 20036
(202) 833-1460 • Fax: (202) 659-9149 • e-mail: pfinfo@policefoundation.org
ABOUT THE
POLICE FOUNDATION
The Police Foundation is a private, independent, not-for-profit organization dedicated to
supporting innovation and improvement in policing through its research, technical assistance, and
communications programs. Established in 1970, the foundation has conducted seminal research in
police behavior, policy, and procedure, and works to transfer to local agencies the best new
information about practices for dealing effectively with a range of important police operational
and administrative concerns. Motivating all of the foundation’s efforts is the goal of efficient,
humane policing that operates within the framework of democratic principles and the highest
ideals of the nation.
BOARD OF DIRECTORS
Chairman
William G. Milliken
President
Hubert Williams
Freda Adler, PhD
Lee P. Brown, PhD
William H. Hudnut III
W. Walter Menninger, MD
Victor H. Palmieri
Henry Ruth
Stanley K. Sheinbaum
Alfred A. Slocum
Sally Suchil
Kathryn J. Whitmire
POLICE
FOUNDATION
OFFICE OF RESEARCH
David Weisburd, PhD
Senior Research Scientist
Rosann Greenspan, PhD
Research Director
Patrick R. Gartin, PhD
Senior Research Associate
David G. Olson, PhD
Senior Research Associate
Edwin E. Hamilton, MA
Senior Research Analyst
Michael Clifton, MA
Research Associate
Jennifer C. Nickisch, MA
Research Associate
Justin Ready, MA
Research Associate
Annette C. Miller, MA
Research Assistant
Rachel Dadusc, BA
Administrative Assistant